Skip to main content
Log in

Accuracy and bias in depressives' judgments for self and other

  • Published:
Cognitive Therapy and Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The inference style of clinical depressives was examined using a proverb interpretation task in which self-generated and other-generated proverb interpretations were judged for their normative adequacy. The correspondence between subjects' judgments of the interpretations and independent ratings based on objective criteria was determined using a profile similarity index. Subject-rater correspondence for depressed and nondepressed subjects was examined to determine whether depressed subjects' judgments could be considered accurate (the depressive realism position; Alloy & Abramson, 1979, 1988) or inaccurate and biased with respect to the self (Beck, 1967, 1976). Results indicated that all subjects were less accurate when judging the self than when judging others. However, this discrepancy was significantly larger for depressed subjects who were more inaccurate in the self condition than the nondepressed subjects. No group differences emerged in the other condition, indicating that depressives were capable of judging the normative adequacy of interpretations. In contrast to Beck's theory, an analysis of error type did not indicate that depressive self-inaccuracy was due to a systematic tendency to distort self-relevant information in a negative direction. However, depressed subjects were less likely than nondepressed subjects to distort other-relevant information in a negative direction. Findings are discussed with respect to issues in the nature and assessment of accuracy and bias, as well as current status of the depressive realism hypothesis.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Ackerman, R., & DeRubeis, R. J. (1991). Is depressive realism real?Clinical Psychology Review, 11, 565–584.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ahrens, A. H., Zeiss, A. M., & Kanfer, R. (1988). Dysphoric deficits in interpersonal standards, self-efficacy, and social comparison.Cognitive Therapy and Research, 12, 53–67.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alloy, L. B., & Abramson, L. Y. (1979). Judgment of contingency in depressed and nondepressed students: Sadder but wiser?Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 108, 441–485.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alloy, L. B., & Abramson, L. Y. (1988). Depressive realism: Four theoretical perspectives. In L. B. Alloy (Ed.),Cognitive processes in depression (pp. 223–265). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alloy, L. B., & Ahrens, A. H. (1987). Depression and pessimism for the future: Biased use of statistically relevant information in predictions for self versus others.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 366–378.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association. (1987).Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed., rev.). Washington, DC: Author.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bargh, J. A., & Tota, M. E. (1988). Context-dependent automatic processing in depression: Accessibility of negative constructs with regard to self but not others.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 925–939.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T. (1967).Depression: Clinical, experimental, and theoretical aspects. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T. (1976).Cognitive therapy and the emotional disorders. New York: International Universities Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, J. D., & Fehr, B. (1990). Self-esteem and perceptions of conveyed impressions: Is negative affectivity associated with greater realism?Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 122–133.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Crocker, J. (1993). Memory for information about others: Effects of self-esteem and feedback.Journal of Research in Personality, 27, 35–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crocker, J., Thompson, L. L., McGraw, K. M., & Ingerman, C. (1987). Downward comparison, prejudice, and evaluations of others: Effects of self-esteem and threat.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 907–916.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cronbach, L. J., & Gleser, G. C. (1953). Assessing similarity between profiles.Psychological Bulletin, 50, 456–473.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dobson, K., & Franche, R. L. (1989). A conceptual and empirical review of the depressive realism hypothesis.Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 21, 419–433.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunning, D., & Story, A. L. (1991). Depression, realism, and the overconfidence effect: Are the sadder wiser when predicting future actions and events?Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 521–532.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dykman, B. M., Abramson, L. Y., Alloy, L. B., & Hartlage, S. (1989). Processing of ambiguous and unambiguous feedback by depressed and nondepressed college students: Schematic biases and their implications for depressive realism.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 431–445.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Endicott, J., & Spitzer, R. L. (1978). A diagnostic interview: The Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia.Archives of General Psychiatry, 35, 837–844.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Golin, S. (1989). Schema congruence and depression: Loss of objectivity in self-and other-inferences.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 98, 495–498.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Golin, S., Terell, F., & Johnson, B. (1977). Depression and the illusion of control.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 86, 440–442.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Golin, S., Terrell, F., Weisz, J., & Drost, P. L. (1979). The illusion of control among depressed patients.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 88, 454–457.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gorham, D. R. (1956). A proverbs test for clinical and experimental use.Psychological Reports, 2 (Monograph Suppl. 1), 1–12.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gotlib, I. H., & Meltzer, S. J. (1987). Depression and the perception of social skill in dyadic interaction.Cognitive Therapy and Research, 11, 41–54.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoehn-Hyde, D., Schlottman, R. S., & Rush, A. J. (1982). Perception of social interactions in depressed psychiatric patients.Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 50, 209–212.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • John, O. P., & Robins, R. W. (1993). Determinants of interjudge agreement on personality traits: The Big Five domains, observability, evaluativeness, and the unique perspective of the self.Journal of Personality, 61, 521–551.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • John, O. P., & Robins, R. W. (1994). Accuracy and bias in self-perception: Individual differences in self-enhancement and the role of narcissism.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 206–219.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kuiper, N. A., & MacDonald, M. R. (1982). Self and other perception in mild depressives.Social Cognition, 1, 223–229.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewinsohn, P. M., Mischel, W., Chaplin, W., & Barton, R. (1980). Social competence and depression: The role of illusory self-perceptions.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 89, 203–212.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lobitz, W. C., & Post, R. D. (1979). Parameters of self-reinforcement and depression.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 88, 33–41.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, D. J., Abramson, L. Y., & Alloy, L. B. (1984). The illusion of control for self and others in depressed and nondepressed college students.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 125–136.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nunnally, J. C. (1978).Psychometric theory. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roth, D., & Rehm, L. P. (1980). Relationships among self-monitoring processes, memory, and depression.Cognitive Therapy and Research, 4, 149–157.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spitzer, R. L., Endicott, J., & Robins, E. (1975).Research Diagnostic Criteria (RDC). New York: New York State Psychiatric Institute, Biometrics Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strack, S., & Coyne, J. C. (1983). Social confirmation of dysphoria: Shared and private reactions.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 798–806.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sweeney, P. D., Shaeffer, D., & Golin, S. (1982). Attributions about self and others in depression.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 8, 37–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tabachnik, N., Crocker, J., & Alloy, L. B. (1983). Depression, social comparison, and the false-consensus effect.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 688–699.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, S. (1989).Positive illusions: Creative self-deception and the healthy mind. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. (1988). Illusion and well-being: A social psychological perspective on mental health.Psychological Bulletin, 103, 193–210.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vazquez, C. V. (1987). Judgment of contingency: Cognitive bias in depressed and nondepressed subjects.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 419–431.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1981).Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Revised, manual, New York: Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Youngren, M. A., & Lewinsohn, P. M. (1980). The functional relation between depression and problematic interpersonal behavior.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 89, 333–341.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

Portions of these data were presented at the 95th annual meeting of the American Psychological Association, New York (1987). We are grateful to Lauren Alloy, Scott Monroe, and several anonymous reviewers for helpful comments on earlier drafts.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Koenig, L.J., Ragin, A.B. & Harrow, M. Accuracy and bias in depressives' judgments for self and other. Cogn Ther Res 19, 505–517 (1995). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02230511

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02230511

Key words

Navigation