Skip to main content
Log in

Language learning and retention in young language-disordered children

  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Simultaneous sign and spoken language training was conducted with young, language-disordered children under standardized training and follow-up conditions with a stringent learning criterion to determine if language learned was stable over time. Twenty-one children between 36 and 86 months with no or nonfunctional language participated in the study. Diagnoses included autism, mental retardation, combined autism and mental retardation, and developmental aphasia. Children completed a mean of 74 signed speech training sessions. Sessions were twice daily, 5 days a week. Follow-up evaluations were made approximately 6 months after training. Of the 21 children, 17 learned at least one word and 7 children learned multiple-word phrases during the training. Most language learned in training was found to be retained at follow-up approximately 6 months later. Gestural imitation, play style, language age, developmental age, and fine motor skills had strong correlations with language learning and retention.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • American Psychiatric Association: (1980).Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: Author.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barrera, R. D., Lobata-Barrera, D., & Sulzer-Azaroff, B. (1980). A simultaneous treatment comparison of three expressive language training programs with a mute autistic child.Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 10, 21–37.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bates, E. (1976).Language and context. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bates, E. (1976).The emergence of symbols: Cognition and communication in infancy. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Benedict, H. (1979). Early lexical development: Comprehension and production.Journal of Child Language, 6, 183–200.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bonvillian, J. D., Nelson, K. E., & Rhyne, J. M. (1981). Sign language and autism.Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 11, 125–137.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, D. T., & Stanley, J. C. (1963).Experimental and quasi-experimental designs for research, Chicago: Rand McNally.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carr, E. G. (1979). Teaching autistic children to use sign language: Some research issues.Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 9, 345–359.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Casey, L. O. (1978). Development of communicative behavior in autistic children: A parent program using manual signs.Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 8, 45–49.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chapman, R. S., & Miller, J. F. (1980). Analyzing language and communication in the child. In R. L. Schiefelbusch (Ed.),Nonspeech language and communication: Analysis and intervention. Baltimore: University Park Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corrigan, R. (1979). Cognitive correlates of language: Differential criteria yield differential results.Child Development, 50, 617–631.

    Google Scholar 

  • Curtiss, S. (1981). Dissociations between language and cognition: Cases and implications.Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 11, 15–30.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Flaharty, R. (1976). EPEC: Evaluation and prescription for exceptional children. In E. R. Ritvo, B. J. Freeman, E. M. Ornitz, & P. Tanguay (Eds.),Autism, diagnosis, current research & management. New York: Spectrum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gaines, R., Mandler, J., & Bryant, P. (1981). Immediate and delayed story recall of hearing and deaf children.Journal of Speech & Hearing Research, 24, 463–469.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garcia, E. E., & DeHaven, E. D. (1974). Use of operant techniques in the establishment and generalization of language: A review and analysis.American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 79, 169–178.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Graham, L. W. (1976). Language programming and intervention. In L. L. Lloyd (Ed.),Communication assessment and intervention strategies. Baltimore: University Park Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kiernan, C. (1983). The use of nonvocal communication techniques with autistic individuals.Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry & Allied Disciplines, 24, 339–375.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lombardino, L. J., Willems, S., & McDonald, J. D. (1981). Critical considerations in total communication and an environmental intervention model for the developmentally delayed.Exceptional Children, 47, 455–461.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lovaas, O. I. (1977).The autistic child. New York: Irvington.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luria, A. R. (1981).Language and cognition. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1962).Play, dreams, and imitation in childhood, New York: W. W. Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Remington, B., & Clarke, S. (1983). Acquistion of expressive signing by autistic children: An evaluation of the effects of simultaneous communication and sign-alone training.Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 16, 315–328.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schaeffer, B. (1980). Teaching signed speech to nonverbal children: Theory and method.Sign Language Studies, 26, 29–63.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shane, H., & Wilbur, R. B. (1980). Predictions of expressive signing potential through assessment of motor capabilities in the severely handicapped.Sign Language Studies, 19, 331–348.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snow, C. (1981). The uses of imitation.Journal of Childhood Language, 8, 205–212.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ungerer, J., & Sigman, M. (1981). Symbolic play and language comprehension in autistic children. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 20, 318–337.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1978).Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. M. Cole, V. John-Steiner, S. Scribner, & E. Souberman (Eds.). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wulz, S. V., Hall, M. K., & Klein, M. D. (1983). A home-centered instructional communication strategy for severely handicapped children.Journal of Speech & Hearing Disorders, 48, 2–10.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

This project was supported by research grant C 008 100457 to Rosslyn Gaines, from the U.S. Department of Education. The authors thank the Mental Retardation and Child Psychiatry unit at the Neuropsychiatric Institute (NPI), University of California, Los Angeles. D. Guthrie and P. Schroth, NPI, provided excellent statistical consultation and computer analyses to the project. Sue Knell and Mirla Geclewicz, NPI, were a great help in the pilot study. Manette McDermott, NPI, kindly supplied additional language age information on each child in the study. The project was graced by the help of a dedicated crew of student research assistants who carried out the communication training and data collection. Our appreciation goes to Gita Amona, Anne-Lauren Arenson, Cara Churchill, Joan Gilbert, Sandy Hansen, Michko Katamini, Scott Krishel, Pam Mabry, Diane Maler, Vicki McWain, Rosela Mendoza, Mona Mosk, Gina Raymond, Carrie Rogers, Mari Takahashi, Sharon Tsunishi, Linda Weinstein, Elaine Wienowksi, Elizabeth Wilhite, Karyn Wood, and Marisa Zarate. Portions of this paper were presented at the Society for Research in Child Development meeting, Detroit, April 1981.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Gaines, R., Leaper, C., Monahan, C. et al. Language learning and retention in young language-disordered children. J Autism Dev Disord 18, 281–296 (1988). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02211953

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02211953

Keywords

Navigation