Advertisement

Prospects

, Volume 26, Issue 1, pp 141–158 | Cite as

Piaget and school education: A socio-cultural challenge

  • Eduardo Martí
Open File Piaget and Education Piaget and Social Aspects of Knowledge

Keywords

School Education 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Allal, L.; Saada-Robert, M. 1992. La métacognition: cadre conceptuel pour l'étude des régulations en situation scolaire.Archives de psychologie (Geneva), vol. 60, no. 235, p. 265–96.Google Scholar
  2. Brown, A. 1990. Domain-specific principles affect learning and transfer in children.Cognitive science (Norwood, NJ), no. 14, p. 107–33.Google Scholar
  3. Brown, A.; Palincsar, A. 1989. Guided, co-operative learning and individual knowledge acquisition.In: Resnick, L. ed.Knowing, learning and instruction: essays in honor of Robert Glaser. Hillsdale, NJ, Erlbaum, p. 393–451.Google Scholar
  4. Carey, S.; Spelke, E. 1994. Domain-specific knowledge and conceptual change.In: Hirschfeld, L.; Gelman, S., eds.Mapping the mind: domain specificity in cognition and culture. Cambridge, MA, Cambridge University Press, p. 169–200.Google Scholar
  5. Chevallard, Y. 1985.La transposition didactique: du savoir savant au savoir enseigné. Grenoble, France, La Pensée sauvage.Google Scholar
  6. Claxton, G. 1991.Educating the inquiring mind. The challenge for school science. London, Harvester.Google Scholar
  7. Coll, C., et al. 1992. Actividad conjunta y habla: una aproximación al estudio de los mecanismos de influencia educativa [Overall activity and language: approach to the study of the mechanisms affecting education].Infancia y aprendizaje (Madrid), nos. 59-60, p. 189–232.Google Scholar
  8. Coll, C.; Onrubia, J. 1994. Temporal dimension and interactive processes in teaching/learning activities: a theoretical and methodological challenge.In: Río, P. del; Alvarez, A.; Wertsch, J. eds.Explorations in socio-cultural studies, vol. 3, p. 107–22. Madrid, Aprendizaje S.L. (Teaching, learning and interaction, edited by N. Mercer, and C. Coll.)Google Scholar
  9. Driver, R.; Guesne, E.; Tiberghien, A. 1985.Children's ideas in science. Milton Keynes, U.K., Open University Press.Google Scholar
  10. Fondation Archives Jean Piaget. 1989.Bibliographie Jean Piaget. Geneva, Fondation Archives Jean Piaget.Google Scholar
  11. Forman, E. 1989. The role of peer interaction in the social construction of mathematical knowledge.International journal of educational research (Kidlington, U.K.), vol. 13, no. 1, p. 55–70.Google Scholar
  12. Gardner, H. 1991.The unschooled mind: how children think and how schools should teach. New York, Basic.Google Scholar
  13. Gelman, R.; Gallistel, C. 1978.The child's understanding of number. Cambridge, MA, Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  14. Goody, J. 1987.The interface between the written and the oral. Cambridge, MA, Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  15. Goustard, M.; Gréco, P.; Matalon, B.; Piaget, J. 1959.La logique des apprentissages. Paris, Presses universitaires de France.Google Scholar
  16. Greenfield, P. 1984.Mind and media: the effects of television, video games and computers. Cambridge, MA, Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  17. Hirschfeld, L.; Gelman, S., eds. 1994.Mapping the mind: domain specificity in cognition and culture. Cambridge, MA, Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  18. Inhelder, B. 1978. De l'approche structurale à l'approche procédurale. Introduction à l'étude des stratégies.Actes du XXIe Congrès international de psychologie. Paris, Presses universitaires de France.Google Scholar
  19. Inhelder, B., et al. 1992.Le cheminement des découvertes de l'enfant. Paris; Neuchâtel, Delachaux & Niestlé.Google Scholar
  20. Karmiloff-Smith, A. 1985. A constructivist approach to modelling linguistic and cognitive development.Archives de psychologie (Geneva), vol. 53, no. 204, p. 113–26.Google Scholar
  21. — 1992.Beyond modularity: a development perspective on cognitive science. Cambridge, Massachusetts, MIT Press.Google Scholar
  22. Keil, F. 1981. Constraints on knowledge and cognitive development.Psychological review (Washington, DC), vol. 88, no. 3, p. 197–227.Google Scholar
  23. Martí, E. 1994a.Mécanismes d'interiorisation/extériorisation des connaissances chez Piaget et Vygotsky. Conférence présentée au 14e Cours avancé des Archives Jean Piaget, Geneva, 19–22 September.Google Scholar
  24. — 1994b. Peer interaction in problem solving. A microgenetic analysis of interpsychological mechanisms.In: Río, P. del; Alvarez, A.; Wertsch, J., eds.Explorations in socio-cultural studies. vol. 3. Teaching, learning and interaction, p. 209–16. Madrid, Aprendizaje S.L.Google Scholar
  25. Morf, A., et al. 1959.L'apprentissage des structures logiques. Paris, Presses universitaires de France.Google Scholar
  26. Olson, D. 1986. Intelligence and literacy: the relationship between intelligence and the technologies of representation and communication. In: Sternberg, R.; Wagner, R., eds.Practical intelligence: nature and origins of competence in the everyday world, p. 338–60. New York, Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  27. Parrat-Dayan, S. 1993. La réception de l'oeuvre de Piaget dans le milieu pédagogique des années 1920–1930.Revue française de pédagogie (Paris), no. 104, p. 73–83.Google Scholar
  28. Piaget, J. 1923.Le langage et la pensée chez l'enfant. Neuchâtel; Delachaux & Niestlé.Google Scholar
  29. — 1924.Le jugement et le raisonnement chez l'enfant. Neuchâtel; Delachaux & Niestlé.Google Scholar
  30. — 1932.Le jugement moral chez l'enfant. Paris, Alcan.Google Scholar
  31. — 1936.La naissance de l'intelligence chez l'enfant. Neuchâtel; Paris, Delachaux & Niestlé.Google Scholar
  32. — 1946.La formation du symbole chez l'enfant. Neuchâtel; Paris, Delachaux & Niestlé.Google Scholar
  33. — 1947.La psychologie de l'intelligence. Paris, Armand Collin.Google Scholar
  34. — 1966. Autobiographie.Cahiers Vilfredo Pareto (Geneva), no. 10, p. 129–59.Google Scholar
  35. — 1968. Le point de vue de Piaget.International journal of psychology (Hove, U.K.), vol. 3, no. 4, p. 281–99.Google Scholar
  36. — 1969.Psychologie et pédagogie. Paris, Denoël.Google Scholar
  37. — 1970.L'épistémologie génétique. Paris Presses universitaires de France.Google Scholar
  38. — 1972.Où va l'éducation? Paris, Denoël/Gonthier.Google Scholar
  39. — 1974.La prise de conscience. Paris, Presses universitaires de France.Google Scholar
  40. — 1975.L'équilibration des structures cognitives: problème central du développement. Paris, Presses universitaires de France.Google Scholar
  41. — 1980a. Recent studies in genetic epistemology.Cahiers de la Fondation Archives Jean Piaget (Geneva), no. 1, p. 3–7.Google Scholar
  42. — 1980b.Les formes élémentaires de la dialectique. Paris, Gallimard.Google Scholar
  43. — 1985. Commentaires sur les remarques critiques de Vygotsky.In: Schneuwly, B.; Bronckart, J.P., eds.Vygotsky aujourd'hui. Neuchâtel, p. 120–37. Paris, Delachaux & Niestlé.Google Scholar
  44. Resnick, L. 1986. The development of mathematical intuition.In: Perlmuter, M., ed.Perspectives on intellectual development: the Minnesota symposium on child development, vol. 19. Hillsdale, NJ, Erlbaum, p. 159–94.Google Scholar
  45. Resnick, L.; Levine, J.; Teasley, S. 1991.Perspectives on socially shared cognition. Washington, DC, American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
  46. Rodrigo, M.; Rodríguez, A.; Marrero, J. 1993.Las teorias implícitas: una aproximación al conocimiento cotidiano [Implicit theories: approach to everyday knowledge]. Madrid, Aprendizaje/Visor.Google Scholar
  47. Rogoff, B. 1990.Apprenticeship in thinking: cognitive development in social context. New York, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  48. Salomon, G.; Leigh, T. 1984. Predispositions about learning from print and television.Journal of communication (New York), no. 34, p. 119–35.Google Scholar
  49. Schoenfeld, A. 1989. Ideas in the air: speculations on small group learning, environmental and cultural influences on cognition and epistemology.International journal of educational research (Kidlington, U.K.), vol. 13, no. 1, p. 71–88.Google Scholar
  50. Sinclair, H., et al. 1985. Constructivisme et psycholinguistique génétique.Archives de psychologie (Geneva), vol. 53, no. 204, p. 37–60.Google Scholar
  51. Teberosky, A. 1993. Introducción: investigación psicológica y educación en dominios específicos [Introduction: psychological investigation and domain-specific education].Substratum (Barcelona, Spain), vol. 1, no. 2, p. 9–19.Google Scholar
  52. Tolchinsky, L.; Karmiloff-Smith, A. 1993. Las restricciones del conocimiento notacional [The restrictions of national knowledge].Infancia y aprendizaje (Madrid), nos. 62-63, p. 19–51.Google Scholar
  53. Van de Veer, R.; Valsiner, J. 1991.Understanding Vygotsky; a quest for synthesis. Cambridge, MA, Blackwell.Google Scholar
  54. Vygotsky, L. 1981a. The instrumental method in psychology.In: Wertsch, J., ed.The concept of activity in Soviet psychology. Armonk, NY, Sharpe.Google Scholar
  55. — 1981b. The genesis of higher mental functions.In: Wertsch, J., ed.The concept of activity in Soviet psychology. Armonk, NY, Sharpe.Google Scholar
  56. Wertsch, J. 1985.Vygotsky and the social formation of mind. Cambridge, MA, Harvard University Press.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© UNESCO 1996

Authors and Affiliations

  • Eduardo Martí
    • 1
    • 2
  1. 1.University of BarcelonaSpain
  2. 2.University of GenevaSwiterland

Personalised recommendations