Skip to main content
Log in

Do tests of nonverbal decoding ability measure sensitivity to nonverbal cues?

  • Published:
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Compared to efficient decoders (who excel on tests of nonverbal decoding ability), inefficient decoders may (1) use a restricted repertoire of nonverbal cues; (2) use the same repertoire but be less sensitive to the cues; or (3) be more sensitive to cues of high informative utility and less sensitive to cues of lesser utility. Subjects were asked to identify romantic heterosexual couples in photographs. Potential cues were generated by think aloud protocols (Study 1); their presence and informative utility were determined through ratings (Study 2). Study 3 estimated cues' influence on “couple” decisions. Decisions were affected by all the informative cues (“proximity,” “contact,” and “context”) with efficient decoders being more sensitive to “proximity” than inefficient decoders. However, inefficient decoders weremore sensitive than efficient decoders to uninformative cues suggesting couples' similarity. Hence, the present test does not measure sensitivity (Hypotheses 1 and 2) but measures sensitivity-tuning (Hypothesis 3).

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abbey, A., & Melby, C. (1986). The effects of nonverbal cues on gender differences in perception of sexual intent.Sex Roles, 15, 283–298.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, N. H. (1981).Methods of information integration theory. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, N. H. (1982).Methods of information integration theory. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Archer, D., & Akert, R. M. (1977). Words and everything else: Verbal and nonverbal cues in social interpretation.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 443–449.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barnes, M. L., & Sternberg, R. J. (1989). Social intelligence and decoding of nonverbal cues.Intelligence, 13, 263–287.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bayes, M. A. (1972). Behavioral cues of interpersonal warmth.Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 39, 333–339.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broun, L. T., & Antony, R. G. (1990). Continuing the search for social intelligence.Personality and Individual Differences, 11, 463–470.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bugental, D. E., Love, L. R., & Gianetto, R. M. (1971). Perfidious feminine faces.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 17, 314–318.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burgoon, J. K., Buller, D. B., Hale, J. L., & DeTurck, M. A. (1984). Relational messages associated with nonverbal behaviors.Human Communication Research, 10, 351–378.

    Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, D., Ervin, C. R., & Lamberth, J. (1970). Continuity between the experimental study of attraction and real-life computer dating.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 16, 157–165.

    Google Scholar 

  • Costanzo, M., & Archer, D. (1989). Interpreting the expressive behavior of others: The Interpersonal Perception Task.Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 13, 225–245.

    Google Scholar 

  • DePaulo, B. M. (1992). Nonverbal behavior and self presentation.Psychological Bulletin, 111, 203–243.

    Google Scholar 

  • Exline, R. V., & Winters, L. C. (1965). Affective relations and mutual glance in dyads. In S. Tomkins & C. Izard (Eds.),Affect, cognition and personality (pp. 319–350) New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guardo, C. J. (1969). Personal space in children.Child Development, 40, 143–151.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J. A. (1978). Gender effects in decoding nonverbal cues.Psychological Bulletin, 85, 845–857.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J. A. (1980). Voice tone and persuasion.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 924–934.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heslin, R., & Boss, D. (1980). Nonverbal intimacy in airport arrival and departure.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 6, 248–252.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelly, F. D. (1972). Communicational significance of therapist proximic cues.Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 39, 345.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kleinke, C. L., Meeker, F. B., & LaFong, C. L. (1974). Effects of gaze, touch and use of name in evaluation of “engaged” couples.Journal of Research in Personality, 7, 368–373.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lorch, R. F., & Meyers, J. L. (1990). Regression analyses in repeated measures data in cognitive research.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 16, 149–157.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maxwell, G. M., Cook, M. W., & Burr, R. (1985). The encoding and decoding of liking from behavioral cues in both auditory and visual channels.Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 9, 239–363.

    Google Scholar 

  • McConaghy, J., & Kirby, N. H. (1987). Analogical reasoning and ability level: An examination of R. J. Sternberg componential method.Intelligence, 11, 137–160.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mehrabian, A. F. S. (1967). Inference of attitudes from nonverbal communication in two channels.Journal of Consulting Psychology, 31(3), 248–252.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mehrabian, A. (1968). Relationship of attitude to seated posture, orientation and distance.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 10, 26–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, J. H., & Aldrich, F. D. (1984).Linear probability: LOGIT and PROBIT models. Beverly Hills: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes.Psychological Review, 84, 231–259.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pastore, R. E., & Scheirer, C. J. (1974). Signal detection theory: Considerations for general application.Psychological Bulletin, 81, 945–958.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenfeld, H. M. (1966). Approval-seeking and approval-avoiding functions of verbal and nonverbal responses in the dyad.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 4, 597–605.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, H. J., Archer, D., & Costanzo, M. (1991). “Just a hunch”: Accuracy and awareness in person perception.Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 15, 3–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. (1985).Beyond I.Q. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J., & Smith, C. (1985). Social intelligence and decoding skill in nonverbal communication.Journal of Social Cognition, 3, 16–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trimboli, A., & Walker, M. B. (1987). Nonverbal dominance in the communication of affect: A myth?Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 11, 180–190.

    Google Scholar 

  • Walker, R. E., & Foley, J. M. (1973). Social intelligence: Its history and measurement.Psychological Reports, 33, 839–864.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Nachshon Meiran.

Additional information

We wish to thank Nehemia Friedland, Yechiel Klar, Naama Meiran, and Gary Diamond for their helpful comments. We also thank the two anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments on the first version of the manuscript.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Meiran, N., Netzer, T., Netzer, S. et al. Do tests of nonverbal decoding ability measure sensitivity to nonverbal cues?. J Nonverbal Behav 18, 223–244 (1994). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02170027

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02170027

Keywords

Navigation