Skip to main content
Log in

Organization of the bilingual lexicon: Categorical versus alphabetic cuing in Russian-English bilinguals

  • Published:
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The mechanism by which Russian native speakers store and retrieve Russian and English words from lexical memory was investigated in two experiments in which fluency of retrieval was compared for categorical and alphabetic cues. We hypothesized that fluency in Russian would be higher for categorical cues but that fluency in English would be higher for alphabetical cues because native language words would have been stored by meaning, while second-language words would have been stored according to dictionary usage. Subjects retrieved more Russian than English words with categorical cues, but their retrieval was the same for the two languages with alphabetic cues. In addition, fluency was greater overall with alphabetical than categorical cues. We attributed the greater fluency in Russian with the categorical cues to the early age at which the concepts were acquired, which made them more accessible in the native language. These findings support the idea that access codes for words may vary depending upon how they are originally learned.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Barchudarov, S. G., Protchenko, I. F., & Skvortsov, L. I. (Eds.). (1979).Orthographical dictionary of Russian language (16th ed.). Moscow: Academy of Science.

    Google Scholar 

  • Battig, W. F., & Montague, W. E. (1969). Category nouns for verbal items in 56 categories: A replication and extension of the Connecticut category norms.Journal of Experimental Psychology, Monograph, 80(3), 1–46.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butters, N., Granholm, E., Salmon, D. P., & Grant, I. (1987). Episodic and semantic memory: A comparison of amnesic and demented patients.Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 9(5), 479–497.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carroll, J. B., & White, M. N. (1973). Word frequency and age of acquisition as determiners of picture-naming latency.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 25, 85–95.

    Google Scholar 

  • Collins, A. M., & Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing.Psychological Review, 82, 407–428.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Bot, K. (1992). A bilingual production model: Levelt's ‘speaking’ model adapted.Applied Linguistics, 13, 1–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Groot, A. M. B. (1992). Determinants of word translation.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 1001–1018.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freedman, J. L., & Loftus, E. F. (1971). Retrieval of words from long-term memory.Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 10, 107–115.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gekoski, W. L., Jacobson, J. Z., & Frazao-Brown, A. P. (1982). Visual masking and linguistic independence in bilinguals.Canadian Journal of Psychology, 36(1), 108–116.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goodglass, H., & Kaplan, E. (1983).The assessment of aphasia and related disorders (2nd ed.). Philadelphia: Lea & Febiger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hanley, J. R., Dewick, H. C., Davies, A. D. M., Playfer, J., & Turnbull, C. (1990). Verbal fluency in Parkinson's disease.Neuropsychologia, 28(7), 737–741.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heindel, W. C., Salmon, D. P., & Butters, N. (1990). Pictorial priming and cued recall in Alzheimer's and Huntington's disease.Brain and Cognition, 13(2), 282–295.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hewett, L. J., Nixon, S. J., Glenn, S. W., & Parsons, O. A. (1991). Verbal fluency deficits in female alcoholics.Journal of Clinical Psychology, 47(5), 716–720.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hodges, J. R., Salmon, D. P., & Butters, N. (1992). Semantic memory impairment in Alzheimer's disease: Failure of access or degraded knowledge?Neuropsychologia, 10(4), 301–314.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jernigan, T. L., & Butters, N. (1989). Neuropsychological and neuroradiological distinctions between Alzheimer's and Huntington's diseases.Neuropsychology, 3, 283–290.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kertesz, A. (1982).Western Aphasia Battery Test manual. New York: Grune & Stratton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kolers, P. A. (1963). Interlingual word associations.Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 2, 291–300.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kolers, P. A., & Gonzalez, E. (1980). Memory for words, synonyms, and translations.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 6, 53–65.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kroll, J. F. (1993). Accessing conceptual representations for words in a second language. In R. Schreuder & B. Weltens (Eds.),The bilingual lexicon (pp. 53–81). Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kroll, J. G., & Curley, J. (1988). Lexical memory in novice bilinguals: The role of concepts in retrieving second language words. In M. Gruneberg, P. Morris, & R. Sykes (Eds.),Practical aspects of memory (Vol. 2, pp. 389–395). London: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kroll, J. F. & Sholl, A. (1992). Lexical and conceptual memory in fluent and nonfluent bilinguals. In R. J. Harris (Ed.),Cognitive processing in bilinguals. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kucera, H., & Francis, W. N. (1967).Computational analysis of present-day American English. Providence: Brown University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loftus, E. F., & Suppes, P. (1972). Structural variables that determine the speed of retrieving words from long-term memory.Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 770–777.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCornack, P. D. (1974). Bilingual linguistic memory: Independence or interdependence; two stores or one? In S. T. Carey (Ed.),Bilingualism, biculturalism and education. Edmonton: University of Alberta.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCormack, P. D. (1977). Bilingual linguistic memory: The independence-interdependence issue revised. In P. A. Hornby (Ed.),Bilingualism. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McLaughlin, B. (1985).Second language acquisition in childhood: School-aged children (Vol. 2). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morrison, C. M., Ellis, A. W., & Quinlan, P. T. (1992). Age of acquisition, not word frequency, affects object naming, not recognition.Memory & Cognition 20, 705–714.

    Google Scholar 

  • O'Brien, M. A. (Ed.). (1954).New Russian-English and English-Russian dictionary (New Orthography). New York: Dover.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paivio, A., Clark, J. M., & Lambert, W. E. (1988). Bilingual dual-coding theory and semantic repetition effects on recall.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 14, 163–172.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paivio, A., & Desrochers, A. (1980). A dual-coding approach to bilingual memory.Canadian Journal of Psychology, 34, 388–399.

    Google Scholar 

  • Palij, M., & Aaronson, D. (1990).A questionnaire for assessing language background differences. Unpublished manuscript.

  • Pressley, M., Levin, J. R., & McDaniel, M. A. (1987). Remembering versus inferring what a word means: Mnemonic and contextual approaches. In M. G. McKeown & M. E. Curtis (Eds.),The nature of vocabulary acquisition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Royer, J. M. (1973). Memory effects for test-like events during acquisition of foreign language vocabulary.Psychological Reports, 32, 195–198.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salmon, D. P., Shimamura, A. P., Butters, N., & Smith, S. (1988). Lexical and semantic priming deficits in patients with Alzheimer's disease.Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 10(4), 477–494.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schreuder, R., & Weltens, B. (Eds.). (1993).The bilingual lexicon. Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snodgrass, J. G. (1984). Concepts and their surface representations.Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 23, 3–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spreen, O., & Benton, A. C. (1969).Neurosensory Center Comprehensive Examination for Aphasia. Victoria, BC: University of Victoria Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thurstone, L., & Thurstone, T. (1943).The Chicago Tests of Primary Mental Abilities. Chicago: Science Research Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Webster's Seventh New Collegiate Dictionary (1967). Springfield, MA: Merriam.

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

This research was supported by Research Grants AFOSR 89-0442 and F49620-92-J-0119 from the U.S. Air Force Office of Scientific Research to Joan Gay Snodgrass. We are indebted to an anomymous reviewer for constructive comments on an earlier version of this article.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Snodgrass, J.G., Tsivkin, S. Organization of the bilingual lexicon: Categorical versus alphabetic cuing in Russian-English bilinguals. J Psycholinguist Res 24, 145–163 (1995). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02143960

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02143960

Keywords

Navigation