Skip to main content
Log in

Extraction of linguistically relevant referential properties through language learning

  • Published:
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In order to identify the role of language in extracting a particular referential property which was involved in a reference situation but which was not expressed as relevant, in the linguistic structure of the subjects' native language, three experiments were performed. Forty subjects were exposed to the same referential situations but to different linguistic structures, each encoding a particular referential property that was, relevant. Miniature artificial languages were used. The referential properties examined were prototypical transitivity as defined by Slobin (1980) and plurality. The results showed that prototypical transitivity was extracted more readily than plurality, even though the subjects were more familiar with the latter property. The discussion emphasized how language serves to promote, the extraction and encoding of linguistically relevant properties that underlie referential situations.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Archer, E. J. (1960). A re-evaluation of the meaningfulness of all possible CVC trigrams.Psychological Monograph,74, (10).

  • Braine, M. D. S. (1966). Learning the positions of words relative to a marker element.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 72, 532–540.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, R. (1973).A first language. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, H. H. (1973). The language-as-fixed-effect fallacy: A critique of language statistics in psychological research.Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 12, 335–359.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, E. J. (1969).Principles of perceptual learning and development. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Golinkoff, R. M., & Gordon, L. (1983). In the beginning was the word: A history of the study of language acquisition. In R. M. Golinkoff (Ed.),The transition from prelinguistic to linguistic communication (pp. 1–25). Hillsdale, New Jersey: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herriot, P. (1970).An introduction to the psychology of language. London: Methuen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. A. (1967). Project grammarama. In G. A. Miller,The psychology of communication: Seven essays (pp. 125–187). New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moeser, S. D., & Bregman, A. S. (1972). The role of reference in the acquisition of a miniature artificial language.Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 759–769.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moeser, S. D., & Bregman A. S. (1973). Imagery and language acquisition.Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 12, 91–98.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moeser, S. D., & Olson, A. J. (1974). The role of reference in children's acquisition of a miniature artifical language.Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 17, 204–218.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mori, K. (1980a). The effect of irrelevant factors in the reference field on the acquisition of a miniature artificial language.Japanese Psychological Research, 22, 72–81.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mori, K. (1980b). [The effect of mistakes in the input on the acquisition of a miniature artificial language].Japanese Journal of Psychology, 51, 179–187. (In Japanese with English summary).

    Google Scholar 

  • Mori, K. (1981). The effect of mistakes in the input on the, acquisition of a miniature artificial language (II): Using a subject-paced procedure.Japanese Psychological Research, 23, 113–117.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mori, K., & Moeser, S. D. (1983). The role of syntax markers and semantic referents in learning an artificial language.Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 22, 701–718.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nagata, H. (1976). Quantitative and qualitative analysis of experience in acquisition of a miniature artificial language.Japanese Psychological Research, 18, 174–182.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nagata, H. (1977). The interaction between syntactic complexity and semantic reference in acquisition of a miniature artificial language.Japanese Psychological Research, 19, 90–96.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nagata, H. (1979). Syntax internalization: A methodological reexamination.Bulletin of the Faculty of Law and Literature, Okayama University, 39, 73–80.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nagata, H. (1981). Effectiveness of word order and grammatical markers as syntactic indicators of, semantic relations.Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 10, 471–486.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nagata, H. (1983a). Effectiveness of word order and grammatical markers as syntactic indicators of semantic relations in opaque input conditions.Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 12, 157–169.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nagata, H. (1983b). Richness of input data as condition of the acquisition of prototypical transitivity in language.American Journal of Psychology, 96, 477–489.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nagata, H. (1984). Effectiveness of word order over grammatical markers as a syntactic indicator of semantic relation in an opaque partial description situation.Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 13, 281–293.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norman, D. A., & Bobrow, D. G. (1975). On data-limited and resource-limited processes.Cognitive Psychology, 7, 44–64.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osgood, C. E. (1980).Lectures on language performance. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reber, A. S. (1967). Implicit learning of artificial grammars.Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 6, 855–863.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reber, A. S. (1969). Transfer of syntactic structures in simple synthetic languages.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81, 115–119.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sapir, E. (1924). The grammarian and his language.American Mercury, 1, 149–155.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saporta, S., Blumenthal, A. L., Lackowski, P., & Reiff, D. G. (1965). Grammatical models and language learning. In S. Rosenberg (Ed.),Directions in psycholinguistics (pp. 15–28) New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schlesinger, I. M. (1977).Production and comprehension of utterances. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Segal, E. M., & Halwes, T. G. (1965). Learning of letter pairs as a prototype of first language learning.Psychonomic Science, 3, 451–452.

    Google Scholar 

  • Segal, E. M., & Halwes, T. G., (1966). The influence of frequency of exposure on the learning of a phrase structural grammar.Psychonomic Science, 4, 157–158.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slobin, D. I. (1973). Cognitive prerequisites for the development of grammar. In C. A. Ferguson & D. I. Slobin (Eds.),Studies of child language development (pp. 175–208). New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slobin, D. I. (1979).Psycholinguistics (2nd ed.), Glenview, Illinois: Scott, Foresman and Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slobin, D. I. (1980). The repeated path between transparency and opacity in language. In U. Bellugi & M. Studdert-Kennedy (Eds.),Signed and spoken language: Biological constraints on linguistic form. Dahlem Konferenzen (pp. 229–243). Weinheim: Verlag Chemi GmbH.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slobin, D. I. (1981). Psychology without linguistics= Language without grammar.Cognition, 10, 275–280.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slobin, D. I. (1982). Universal and particular in the acquisition of language. In E. Wanner & L. R. Gleitman (Eds.),Language, acquisition: The state of the art (pp. 128–170). London: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, K. H. (1965). Mediation and position learning in the recall of structured letter pairs.Psychonomic Science, 2, 293–294.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, K. H. (1966a). Grammatical intrusions, in the recall of structured letter pairs: Mediated transfer or position learning?Journal of Experimental Psychology, 72, 580–588.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, K. H. (1966b). Grammatical intrusion in the free recall of, structured letter pairs.Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 5, 447–454.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, K. H. (1969). Learning co-occurrence restrictions: Rule induction or rote learning?Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 8, 319–321.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steinberg, D. D. (1982).Psycholinguistics: Language, mind, and world. New York: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

Deep gratitude is due to Prof. D. D. Steinberg for his cogent and critical reading of the earlier version of the paper.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Nagata, H. Extraction of linguistically relevant referential properties through language learning. J Psycholinguist Res 14, 447–464 (1985). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01666720

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01666720

Keywords

Navigation