Skip to main content
Log in

Reasoning strategies used by students to solve stoichiometry problems and its relationship to alternative conceptions, prior knowledge, and cognitive variables

  • Published:
Journal of Science Education and Technology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Achievement in science depends on a series of factors that characterize the cognitive abilities of the students and the complex interactions between these factors and the environment that intervenes in the formation of students' background. The objective of this study is to: a) investigate reasoning strategies students use in solving stoichiometric problems; b) explore the relation between these strategies and alternative conceptions, prior knowledge and cognitive variables; and c) interpret the results within an epistemological framework. Results obtained show how stoichiometric relations produce conflicting situations for students, leading to conceptual misunderstanding of concepts, such as mass, atoms and moles. The wide variety of strategies used by students attest to the presence of competing and conflicting frameworks (progressive transitions, cf. Lakatos, 1970), leading to greater conceptual understanding. It is concluded that the methodology developed in this study (based on a series of closely related probing questions, generally requiring no calculations, that elicit student conceptual understanding to varying degrees within an intact classroom context) was influential in improving student performance. This improvement in performance, however, does not necessarily affect students' hard core of beliefs.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Anamuah-Mensah, J. (1986). Cognitive strategies used by chemistry students to solve volumetric analysis problems.Journal of Research in Science Teaching 23, 759–769.

    Google Scholar 

  • Atwater, M. M., and Alick, B. (1990). Cognitive development and problem solving of Afro-American students in chemistry.Journal of Research in Science Teaching 27, 157–172.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ben-Zvi, R., Eylon, B. and Silberstein, J. (1986). Is an atom of copper malleable?Journal of Chemical Education 63, 64–66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bitner, B. L. (1991). Formal operational reasoning modes: Predictors of critical thinking abilities and grades assigned by teachers in science and mathematics for students in grades nine through twelve.Journal of Research in Science Teaching 28, 265–274.

    Google Scholar 

  • BouJaoude, S. B. (1992). The relationship between students' learning strategies and the change in their misunderstandings during a high school chemistry course.Journal of Research in Science Teaching 29, 687–699.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burbules, N. C., and Linn, M. C. (1991). Science education and philosophy of science: Congruence or contradiction?International Journal of Science Education 13, 227–241.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chinn, C. A., and Brewer W. F. (1993). The role of anomalous data in knowledge acquisition: A theoretical framework and implications for science instruction.Review of Educational Research 63(1), 1–49.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clement, J. (1982). Students' preconceptions in introductory mechanics.American Journal of Physics 50, 60–71.

    Google Scholar 

  • Driver, R., and Easley, J. (1978). Pupils and paradigms: A review of literature related to concept development in adolescent science students.Studies in Science Education 5, 61–84.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duschl, R. A., and Gitomer, D. H. (1991). Epistemological perspectives on conceptual change: Implications for educational practice.Journal of Research in Science Teaching 28, 839–858.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gabel, D. L., Sherwood, R. D., and Enochs, L. (1984). Problemsolving skills of high school chemistry students.Journal of Research in Science Teaching 21, 221–233.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert, J. K., and Swift, D. J. (1985). Towards a Lakatosian analysis of the Piagetian and alternative conceptions research programs.Science Education 69, 681–696.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert, J. K., and Watts, D. M. (1983). Concepts, misconceptions and alternative conceptions: Changing perspectives in science education.Studies in Science Education 10, 61–98.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hafner, B., and Stewart, J. (1989). A comment on predicting genetics achievement in nonmajors college biology.Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 26(6), 551–553.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haidar, A. H., and Abraham, M. R. (1991). A comparison of applied and theoretical knowledge of concepts based on the particulate nature of matter.Journal of Research in Science Teaching 28, 919–938.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnstone, A. H., and El-Banna, H. (1986). Capacities, demands, and processes—a predictive model for science education.Education in Chemistry 23, 80–84.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnstone, A. H., and El-Naeme, F. F. (1991). Room for scientific thought?International Journal of Science Education 13, 187–192.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lakatos, I. (1970). Falsification and the methodology of scientific research programs. In Lakatos, I., and Musgrave, A. (Eds.),Criticism and the growth of knowledge. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK, pp. 91–196.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lawson, A. E. (1983). Predicting science achievement: the role of developmental level, disembedding ability, mental capacity, prior knowledge, and beliefs,Journal of Research in Science Teaching 20, 117–129.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lawson, A. E. (1988). The acquisition of biological knowledge during childhood: cognitive conflict or tabula rasa?Journal of Research in Science Teaching 25, 185–199.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lawson, A. E., McElrath, C. B., Burton, M. S., James, B. D., Doyle, R. P., Woodward, S. L., Kellerman, L., and Snyder, J. D. (1991). Hypothetico-deductive reasoning skill and concept acquisition: testing a constructivist hypothesis.Journal of Research in Science Teaching 28, 953–970.

    Google Scholar 

  • Linn, M. C., and Songer, N. B. (1991). Teaching thermodynamics to middle school students: What are appropriate cognitive demands?Journal of Research in Science Teaching 28, 885–918.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lythcott, J., and Duschl, R. (1990). Qualitative research: from methods to conclusions.Science Education 74, 445–460.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCloskey, M., Caramazza, A., and Green, B. (1980). Curvilinear motion in the absence of external forces: naive beliefs about the motion of objects.Science 210, 1139–1141.

    Google Scholar 

  • McDermott, L. C. (1984). Research on conceptual understanding in mechanics.Physics Today 37, 24–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nakhleh, M. B. (1992). Why some students don't learn chemistry.Journal of Chemical Education 69, 191–196.

    Google Scholar 

  • Niaz, M. (1988). Manipulation of M-demand of chemistry problems and its effect on student performance: a neo-Piagetian study.Journal of Research in Science Teaching 28 643–657.

    Google Scholar 

  • Niaz, M. (1992). From Piaget's epistemic subject to Pascual-Leone's metasubject: Epistemic transition in the constructivist-rationalist theory of cognitive development.International Journal of Psychology 27, 443–457.

    Google Scholar 

  • Niaz, M. (1993a). Problem solving in science: the role of environment, creativity, developmental level, mental capacity, and cognitive style.Journal of College Science Teaching 23, 18–23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Niaz, M. (1993b). If Piaget's epistemic subject is dead, shall we bury the scientific research methodology of idealization?Journal of Research in Science Teaching 30, 809–812.

    Google Scholar 

  • Niaz, M. (1993c). Progressive ‘problemshifts’, between different research programs in science education: A Lakatosian perspective.Journal of Research in Science Teaching 30, 757–765.

    Google Scholar 

  • Niaz, M. (1993d). Competing research programs in science education: A Lakatosian interpretation,Interchange 24, 181–190.

    Google Scholar 

  • Niaz, M. (1994a). Mas allá del positivismo: Una interpretación Lakatosiana de la enseñanza de las ciencias.Enseñanza de las ciencias 12, 97–100.

    Google Scholar 

  • Niaz, M. (1994b). Enhancing thinking skills: Domain specific/domain general strategies—A dilemma for science education.Instructional Science 22, 413–422.

    Google Scholar 

  • Niaz, M. (1994c). Pascual-Leone's theory of constructive operators as an explanatory construct in cognitive development and science achievement.Educational Psychology 14(1), 23–43.

    Google Scholar 

  • Niaz, M. (1995). Progressive transitions from algorithmic to conceptual understanding in student ability to solve chemistry problems: A Lakatosian interpretation.Science Education 79, 19–36.

    Google Scholar 

  • Niaz, M. (in press). Reasoning strategies of students in solving chemistry problems as a function of developmental level, functional M-capacity, and disembedding ability.International Journal of Science Education.

  • Niaz, M., and Lawson, A. E. (1985). Balancing chemical equations: the role of developmental level and mental capacity.Journal of Research in Science Teaching 22, 41–51.

    Google Scholar 

  • Niaz, M., and Robinson, W. R. (1992). Manipulation of logical structure of chemistry problems and its effect on student performance.Journal of Research in Science Teaching 29, 211–226.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nurrenbern, S. C., and Pickering, M. (1987). Concept learning versus problem solving: is there a difference?Journal of Chemical Education 64, 508–510.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pascual-Leone, J. (1987). Organismic processes for neo-Piagetian theories: A dialectical causal account of cognitive development.International Journal of Psychology 22, 531–570.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pascual-Leone, J., and Burtis, P. J. (1974).FIT: Figural Intersection Test, a group measure of M—capacity. Unpublished manuscript, York University, Toronto, Ontario.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piburn, M. D. (1990). Reasoning about logical propositions and success in science.Journal of Research in Science Teaching 27, 887–900.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roadrangka, V., Yeany, R. H., and Padilla, M. J. (1983).The construction and validation of Group Assessment of Logical Thinking (GALT). Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the National Association for Research in Science Teaching (NARST), April, Dallas, Texas.

  • Roth, W. M. (1990). Neo-Piagetian predictors of achievement in science.Journal of Research in Science Teaching 27, 509–521.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rowell, J. A., and Dawson, C. J. (1985). Equilibration, conflict and instruction: A new class-oriented perspective.European Journal of Science Education 7, 331–344.

    Google Scholar 

  • Staver, J., and Jacks, T. (1988). The influence of cognitive reasoning level, cognitive restructuring ability, disembedding ability, working memory capacity, and prior knowledge on students' performance on balancing equations by inspection.Journal of Research in Science Teaching 25, 763–775.

    Google Scholar 

  • Staver, J. R., and Lumpe, A. T. (1993). A content analysis of the presentation of the mole concept in chemistry textbooks.Journal of Research in Science Teaching 30, 321–337.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strike, K. A., and Posner, G. J. (1992). A revisionist, theory of conceptual change. In Duschl, R. A., and Hamilton, R. J. (Eds.)Philosophy of Science, Cognitive Psychology, and Educational Theory in Practice. State University of New York Press, Albany, NY, pp. 147–176.

    Google Scholar 

  • Witkin, H. A., Oltman, P. K., Raskin, E., and Karp, S. A. (1971).A Manual for the Embedded Figures Tests. Consulting Psychologists Press, Palo Alto, CA.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

de Astudillo, L.R., Niaz, M. Reasoning strategies used by students to solve stoichiometry problems and its relationship to alternative conceptions, prior knowledge, and cognitive variables. J Sci Educ Technol 5, 131–140 (1996). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01575153

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01575153

Key Words

Navigation