Abstract
The relationship of gestural joint attention behaviors and the development of effective communication skills in autism and developmental language delay (DLD) was investigated. Autistic and DLD children matched for MA and MLU were compared on measures of gestural joint attention behavior, personal pronoun use, and spontaneous communicative behavior. DLD children responded correctly to joint attention interactions more often than autistic children, and their spontaneous gestural behavior was more communicative and developmental advanced. Correct production of “I/you” pronouns was related to number of spontaneous initiations for autistic but not for DLD children. Measures of spontaneous joint attention behaviors were in general not related to MA, CA, or MLU for either group. DLD children's performance suggests no special impairment of joint attention skills, whereas autistic children's performance suggests a joint attention deficit in addition to a language deficit.
Similar content being viewed by others
Explore related subjects
Discover the latest articles, news and stories from top researchers in related subjects.References
Arthur, G. (1980).The Arthur adaptation of the Leiter International Performance Scale. Chicago: Stoelting.
Bartak, L., & Rutter, M. (1974). Use of personal pronouns by autistic children.Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 4, 217–222.
Bartak, L., Rutter, M., & Cox, A. (1975). A comparative study of infantile autism and specific developmental receptive language disorder. I. The children.British Journal of Psychiatry, 126, 127–145.
Bartolucci, G., & Albers, R. J. (1974). Deictic categories in the language of autistic children.Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 4, 131–134.
Bloom, L., & Lahey, M. (1978).Language development and language disorders. New York: Wiley.
Bruner, R. (1975). From communication to language—a psychological perspective.Cognition, 3, 255–287.
Cantwell, D., Barker, L., & Rutter, M. (1978). A comparative study of infantile autism and developmental receptive language disorder: IV Syntactical and functional analysis of language.Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 19, 351–362.
Churchill, D. (1972). The relation of infantile autism and early childhood schizophrenia to developmental language disorders of childhood.Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 2, 182–197.
Clark, E. V. (1978). From gesture to word: On the natural history of deixis in language acquisition. In J. S. Bruner & A. Garton (Eds.),Human growth and development: Wolfson College Lectures 1976. London: Oxford University Press.
Cunningham, M. (1968). A comparison of the language of psychotic and non-psychotic children who are mentally retarded.Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 9, 229–244.
Fay, W. H. (1971). Normal and autistic pronouns.Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 36, 242–260.
Fay, W. H. (1982). The development of yes and no answers in autistic children.Topics in Language Disorders, 3, 24–32.
Fay, W. H., & Schuler, A. L. (1980).Emerging language in autistic children. Baltimore: University Park Press.
Flaherty, R. (1976). EPEC: Evaluation and prescription for exceptional children. In E. Ritvo (Ed.),Autism: Diagnosis, current research and management (pp.35–39). New York: Spectrum.
Hermelin, B. (1968). Recent experimental research. In P. J. Mittler (Ed.),Aspects of autism. London: British Psychological Society.
Hermelin, B. (1976). Coding the sense modalities. In L. Wing (Ed.),Early childhood autism: Clinical educational and social aspects (2nd ed.) (pp. 135–168). Oxford: Pergamon Press.
Lock, A. (Ed.). (1978).Action, gesture, and symbol: The emergence of language. New York: Academic Press.
Loveland, K. A. (1984). Learning about points of view: The acquisition ofI/you.Journal of Child Language, 11(3), 535–556.
Prior, M. M. & Hall, L. (1979). Comprehension of transitive and intransitive phrases by autistic, retarded, and normal children.Journal of Communication Disorders, 17, 103–111.
Ricks, D. & Wing, L. (1975). Language, communication, and the use of symbols in normal and autistic children.Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 5, 191–221.
Rutter, M. (1965). Speech disorders in a series of autistic children. In A. W. Franklin (Ed.),Children with communication problems. London: Pitman.
Shapiro, T., Chiarandini, I. & Fish, B. (1974). Thirty severely disturbed children: Evaluation of their language development or classification and prognosis.Archives of General Psychiatry, 42, 171–182.
Watson, L. R., & Lord, C. (1982). Developing a social communication curriculum for autistic students.Topics in Language Disorders, 3, 1–9.
Wetherby, A., & Prutting, C. (1984). Profiles of communicative and cognitive-social abilities in autistic children.Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 27, 364–377.
Wing, L. (1969). The handicaps of autistic children—a comparative study.Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 10, 1–40.
Wing, L. (1979). Differentiation of retardation and autism from specific communication disorders.Child Care Health Development, 5, 57–68.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Additional information
This work was supported in part by a grant to the authors from the National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disorders and Stroke, No. NS18448. The authors would like to express thanks to David Breiger, Sheryl Hughes, Debra Davis, Sharon Hall, Robin McEvoy, and JoLynn Archer for their valuable help in data collection and coding, to David Francis and Sheryl Hughes for help in statistical analysis, and to Betty Redwine for her work in scheduling.
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Loveland, K.A., Landry, S.H. Joint attention and language in autism and developmental language delay. J Autism Dev Disord 16, 335–349 (1986). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01531663
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01531663