Skip to main content
Log in

The development of personal space: Proxemic behavior of children 6 through 16

  • Published:
Human Ecology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Observations were made of the personal space behaviors of same-sex pairs of children, aged 6–16. Results indicated that children used more space as they grew older and that adult proxemic behaviors were acquired by age 12. While in the younger children no sex differences were present for the proxemic behaviors of distance and body orientation, males were found by early adolescence to stand farther apart and at greater angles than females. The development of personal space and of sex differences in these behaviors is discussed in the context of social learning.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Aiello, J. R. (1972). A test of equilibrium theory: Visual interaction in relation to orientation, distance and sex of interactants.Psychon. Sci. 27: 335–336.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aiello, J. R. (1973). Gaze direction during interaction. Paper presented at the Eastern Psychological Association Convention, Washington, D.C., May.

  • Aiello, J. R., and Cooper, R. E. (1972). Use of personal space as a function of social affect. InProceedings of the 80th Annual Convetion. American Psychological Association, Washington, D.C., pp. 207–208.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aiello, J. R., and Jones, S. E. (1971). A field study of the proxemic behavior of young school children in three subcultural groups.J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 19: 351–356.

    Google Scholar 

  • Argyle, M., and Dean, J. (1965). Eye contact, distance, and affiliation.Sociometry 28: 289–304.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ashear, V., and Snortum, J. R. (1971). Eye contact in children as a function of age, sex, and social and intellectual variables.Develop. Psychol. 4: 479.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bass, N., and Weinstein, M. (1971). Early development of interpersonal distance in children.Can. J. Behav. Sci. 3: 368–372.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baxter, J. C. (1970). Interpersonal spacing in natural settings.Sociometry 33: 444–456.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, R. (1970). Spatial meanings and the properties of the environment. In Proshansky, H., Ittelson, W., and Rivlin, L. (eds.),Environmental Psychology: Man and His Physical Setting, Holt, Rinehart and Winston, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bloom, R., Harvey, H., and Howells, G. (1973). The development of interpersonal distance in children and adolescents. Paper presented at the Western Psychological Association, Anaheim, California, April.

  • Booream, C. D., and Flowers, J. V. (1972). Reduction of anxiety and personal space as a function of assertion training with severely disturbed neuropsychiatrie inpatients.Psychol. Rep. 30: 923–929.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, R., and Price, M. A. (1966). The generality of social schemas.J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 3: 589–592.

    Google Scholar 

  • Estes, B. W., and Rush, D. (1971). Social schemas: A developmental study.J. Psychol. 78: 119–123.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, C. (1968). Social schemas: Response sets or perceptual meanings?J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 10: 8–14.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fry, A. M., and Willis, F. N. (1971). Invasion of personal space as function of the age of the invader.Psychol. Rec. 21: 385–389.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg, G. H., Keisler, C., and Collins, B. (1969). Visual behavior and face-to-face distance during interaction.Sociometry 32: 43–53.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guardo, C. (1969). Personal space in children.Child Develop. 40: 143–151.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, E. T. (1959).The Silent Language, Fawcett, Greenwich, Conn.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, E. T. (1963). A system for the notation of proxemic behavior.Am. Anthropologist 65: 1003–1026.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, E. T. (1966).The Hidden Dimension, Doubleday, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hobbs, N. (1966). Helping disturbed children: Psychological and ecological strategies.Am. Psychologist 21: 1105–1115.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, S. E. (1971). A comparative proxemics analysis of dyadic interaction in selected subcultures of New York City.J. Soc. Psychol. 34: 35–44.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, S. E., and Aiello, J. R. (1973). Proxemic behavior of black and white first, third, and fifth grade children.J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 25: 21–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Katz, N. (1937).Animals and Men, Longmans, Green, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klopfer, P. H., and Hailman, J. P. (1967).An Introduction to Animal Behavior, Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, N.J.

    Google Scholar 

  • Little, K. B. (1965). Personal space.J. Exptl. Soc. Psychol 1: 237–247.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maccoby, E. E., and Jacklin, C. N. (1971). Sex differences and their implications for sex roles. Paper presented at the American Psychological Association Convention, Washington, D.C., September.

  • Maccoby, E. E., and Jacklin, C. N. (1973). Stress, activity, and proximity seeking: Sex differences in the year-old child.Child Develop. 44: 34–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • McBride, G., King, H. G., and James, J. W. (1965). Social proximity effects on galvanic skin response in adult humans.J. Psychol. 61: 153–157.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mehrabian, A. (1968). Relationship of attitudes to seated posture, orientation, and distance.J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 10: 26–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meisels, M., and Guardo, C. J. (1969). Development of personal space schemata.Child Develop. 40: 1167–1178.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mueller, E. (1971). Origins of success and failure in children's spontaneous communication. InProceedings of the 79th Annual Convention, American Psychological Association, Washington, D.C., pp. 153–154.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scheflen, A. E. (1965). Quasi-courtship behavior in psychotherapy.Psychiatry 28: 245–257.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sommer, R. (1959). Studies in personal space.Sociometry 22: 247–260.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sommer, R. (1969).Personal Space: The Behavioral Basis of Design, Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, N.J.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolor, A. (1968). Psychological distance in disturbed and normal children.Psychol. Rep. 23: 695–701.

    Google Scholar 

  • Willis, N. (1966). Initial speaking distance as a function of the speakers' relationship.Psychon. Sci. 5: 221–222.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winer, B. J. (1962).Statistical Principles in Experimental Design, McGraw-Hill, New York.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

This research was supported in part by NIMH Grant MH-10779-04 and a grant to J. R. A. from the Research Council of Rutgers University.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Aiello, J.R., De Carlo Aiello, T. The development of personal space: Proxemic behavior of children 6 through 16. Hum Ecol 2, 177–189 (1974). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01531420

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01531420

Keywords

Navigation