Skip to main content
Log in

Categorizing and combining theories of cognitive development and learning

  • Published:
Educational Psychology Review Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The present paper considers the centrality of meta-theoretical belief systems (MTBSs) for theories of cognitive development and learning. It is argued that MTBSs guide and constrain the construction of theoretical models and developmental mechanisms. Moreover, it is shown how MTBSs can be effectively used to categorize theories and to address the issue of whether theories of cognitive development and learning can be coherently combined to form larger, more comprehensive theories. The paper proceeds by first defining the nature of theories and then defining eight major MTBSs: nativism, empiricism, constructivism, structuralism, functionalism, behaviorism, neobehaviorism, and cognitivism. Following this exposition, it is shown how MTBSs underlie model construction. Next, the issue of whether theories can be combined is discussed in terms of the logical compatibility of their underlying MTBSs. Then, two contemporary theories which combine constructs from several different theoretical perspectives are described to illustrate how theories can be combined coherently. Finally, implications are drawn.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Anderson, J. R. (1982). Acquisition of cognitive skill.Psychol. Rev. 89: 369–406.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, J. R. (1983).The Architecture of Cognition Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ashcraft, M. H. (1982). The development of mental arithmetic: A chronometric approach.Devel. Rev. 2: 213–236.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baker, C. L., and McCarthy, J. J. (1981).The Logical Problem of Language Acquisition MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (1977).Social Learning Theory Prentice Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bates, E., and MacWhinney, B. (1982). Functionalist approaches to grammar. In Wanner, E., and Gleitman, L. (eds.),Language Acquisition: The State of the Art Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bates, E., and MacWhinney, B. (1987). Competition, variation, and language learning. In MacWhinney, B. (ed.),Mechanisms of Language Acquisition Hillsdale, NJ, Erlbaum, pp. 157–194.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beilin, H. (1983). The new functionalism and Piaget's program. In E. K. Scholnick (ed.),New Trends in Conceptual Representation: Challenges to Piaget's Theory? Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bereiter, C. (1985). Toward a solution of the learning paradox.Rev. Educat. Res. 55: 201–226.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bereiter, C. (1991). Implications of connectionism for thinking about rules.Educat. Res. 20: 10–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berlyne, D. E. (1965).Structure and Direction in Thinking Wiley, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bindra, D. (1984). Cognition: Its origin and future in psychology. In Royce, J. R., and Mos, L. P. (eds.),Annals of Theoretical Psychology (Vol. 1), Plenum, New York, pp. 1–29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Block, N. (1986). Advertisement for a semantics for psychology. In French, P. A., Uehling, T. E., and Wettstein, H. K. (eds.),Midwest Studies in Philosophy (Vol. X), University of Minnesota Press, Minneapolis, MN, pp. 615–678.

    Google Scholar 

  • Block, N. (1990). The computational model of mind. In Osherson, D. N. and Smith, E. E. (eds.),Thinking: An Invitation to Cognitive Science (Vol. 3), MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp. 247–289.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brainerd, C. J. (1978). The stage question in cognitive-developmental theory.Brain Behav. Sci. 2: 173–213.

    Google Scholar 

  • Byrnes, J. P. (1988). Formal operations: A systematic reformulation.Devel. Rev. 8: 66–87.

    Google Scholar 

  • Byrnes, J. P. (1992). Meaningful logic: Developmental perspectives. In Beilin, H. and Pufall, P. (eds.),Piaget's theory: Prospects and Possibilities (pp. 163–183). Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Byrnes, J. P., and Wasik, B. A. (1991). the role of conceptual knowledge in mathematical procedural learning.Devel. Psychol. 27: 777–786.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carey, S. (1985).Conceptual Change in Childhood MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Case, R. (1985).Intellectual Development: A Systematic Reinterpretation Academic Press, Orlando, FL.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cassirer, E. (1955).The Philosophy of Symbolic Forms Yale University Press, New Haven.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chapman, M. (1988).Constructive Evolution: Origin and Development of Piaget's Thought, Cambridge, New York.

  • Chomsky, N. (1959). Review of Skinner (1957).Language 35: 26–58.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1965).Aspects of the Theory of Syntax MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Collins, A. M., and Quillian, M. R. (1969). Retrieval time from semantic memory.J. Verb. Learn. Verb. Behav. 8: 240–247.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, K. W. (1980). A theory of cognitive development: The control and construction of hierarchies of skills.Psychol. Rev. 87: 447–531.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, J. H. (1963).The Developmental Psychology of Jean Piaget D. Van Nostrand, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, J. H. (1970). Concept development. In Mussen, P. H. (ed.),Carmichael's Manual of Child Psychology (3rd Ed., Vol. I), Wiley, New York, pp. 983–1060.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. A. (1975).The Language of Thought Crowell, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. A. (1981).Representations MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. A., and Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1988). Connectionism and cognitive architecture: A critical analysis.Cognition 28: 3–71.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gardner, H. (1985).The Mind's New Science: The History of the Cognitive Revolution Basic Books, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garrett, M. F. (1990). Sentence processing. In Osherson, D. N., and Lasnik, H. (eds.),Language: An Invitation to Cognitive Science (Vol. 1), MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp. 133–174.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gelman, R. and Baillargeon, R. (1983). A review of some Piagetian concepts. In Mussen, P. H. (ed.),Handbook of Child Psychology (Vol. III), Wiley, New York, pp. 167–230.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, E. J. (1982). The concept of affordances in development: The renascence of functionalism. In Collins, W. A. (ed.),The Concept of Development: The Minnesota Symposium on Child Psychology (Vol. 15), Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 55–81.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ginsburg, H. P., and Opper, S. (1988).Piaget's Theory of Intellectual Development Prentice Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greeno, J. G. (1983). Forms of understanding in mathematical problem solving. In Paris, S. G., Olson, G. M., and Stevenson, H. W. (Eds.),Learning and Motivation in the Classroom Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 83–111.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harlow, H. F. (1949). The formation of learning sets.Psychol. Rev. 56: 51–65.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hiedbreder, E. (1933).Seven Psychologies Prentice Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holyoak, K. J., and Thagard, P. (1989). Analogical mapping by constraint satisfaction.Cogn. Sci. 13: 295–355.

    Google Scholar 

  • House, B., and Zeaman, D. (1962). Reversal and nonreversal shifts in discrimination learning in retardates.J. Exp. Psychol. 63: 444–451.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. (1930). Knowledge and purpose as habit mechanisms.Psychol. Rev. 37: 511–525.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. (1931). Goal attraction and directing ideas conceived of as habit phenomena.Psychol. Rev. 38: 487–506.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hume, D. (1748/1955).An Inquiry Concerning Human Understanding The Bobbs-Merrill Company, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Inhelder, B., and de Caprona, D. (1990). The role and meaning of structures in genetic epistempology. In Overton, W. F. (ed.),Reasoning, Necessity and Logic: Developmental Perspectives Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 33–46.

    Google Scholar 

  • Inhelder, B., and Piaget, J. (1958).The Growth of Logical Thinking from Childhood to Adolescence Basic Books, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Inhelder, B., and Piaget, J. (1964).The Early Growth of Logic in the Child W. W. Norton & Co., New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keating, D. P. (1990). Structuralism, deconstruction, reconstruction: The limits of reasoning. In Overton, W. F. (ed.),Reasoning, Necessity, and Logic: Developmental Perspectives Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 299–319.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keil, F. C. (1981). Constraints on knowledge and cognitive development.Psychol. Rev. 88: 197–227.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kendler, H. H., and Kendler, T. S. (1968). Mediation and conceptual behavior. In Spence, J. W., and Spence, J. T. (eds.),The Psychology of Learning and Motivation (Vol. 2), Academic Press, Orlando, FL, pp. 197–244.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kessen, W. (1983). The child and other cultural inventions. In Kessel, F. S., and Siegel, A. S. (eds.),The Child and Other Cultural Inventions Praeger, New York, pp. 26–39.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kessen, W. (1984). Introduction: The end of the age of development. In Sternberg, R. J. (ed.),Mechanisms of Cognitive Development W. H. Freeman, New York, pp. 1–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klahr, D., and Wallace, J. G. (1976).Cognitive Development: An Information Processing View Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koch, S. (1978). Psychology and the future.Am. Psychol. 33: 631–647.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kosslyn, S. M., Pick, H. L., and Fariello, G. R. (1974). Cognitive maps in children and men.Child Devel. 45: 707–716.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, D. (1989). Children and adults as intuitive scientists.Psychol. Rev. 96: 674–689.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, T. (1977).The Essential Tension University of Chicago Press, Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G. (1987).Women, Fire and Dangerous Things University of Chicago Press, Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levi-Strauss, C. (1966).The Savage Mind University of Chicago Press, Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lycan, W. G. (1987).Consciousness MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, J. M., and Johnson, N. S. (1977). Remembrance of things parsed: Story structure and recall.Cogn. Psychol. 9: 111–151.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. A., Galanter, E., and Pribram, K. H. (1960).Plans and the Structure of Behavior Holt, Rinehart & Winston, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Montangero, J. (1985).Genetic Epistemology: Yesterday and Today City University of New York Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nagel, E. (1961).The Structure of Science Harcourt, Brace & World, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neisser, U. (1967).Cognitive Psychology Appleton-Century-Crofts, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newell, A., and Simon, H. A. (1972).Human Problem Solving Prentice Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Novak, J., and Gowin, D. B. (1984).Learning to Learn Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ornstein, P. A., and Naus, M. J. (1985). Effects of the knowledge base on children's strategies. In Reese, H. W. (ed.),Advances in Child Development and Behavior (Vol. 19), Academic Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ornstein, P. A., Naus, M. J., and Liberty, C. (1975). Rehearsal and organization processes in children's memory.Child Devel. 26: 818–830.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osherson, D. N. (1976).Logical Abilities in Children (Vol. 4): Reasoning and Concepts Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Overton, W. F. (1984). World views and their influence on psychological theory and research: Kuhn-Lakatos-Laudan. In Reese, H. W. (ed.),Advances in Child Development (Vol. 18), Academic Press, Orlando, FL, pp. 191–226.

    Google Scholar 

  • Overton, W. F. (1990). Competence and procedures: Constraints on the development of logical reasoning. In Overton, W. F. (eds.),Reasoning, Necessity and Logic: Developmental Perspectives Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 1–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paris, S. G. (1978). The development of inference and transformation as memory operations. In Ornstein, P. A. (ed.),Memory Development in Children Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paris, S. G., and Byrnes, J. P. (1989). The constructivist approach to self-regulation and learning in the classroom. In Zimmerman, B. and Schunk, D. (eds.),Self-Regulated Learning: Theory, Research and Practice Springer-Verlag, New York, pp. 169–200.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1952).The Origins of Intelligence in Children International Universities Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1967).Six Psychological Studies Vintage Books, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1970).Structuralism Harper & Row, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1985).The Equilibration of Cognitive Structures University of Chicago Press, Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J., and Garcia, R. (1974).Understanding Causality Basic Books, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J., and Inhelder, B. (1973).Memory and Intelligence Routledge & Kegan Paul, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J., Grize, J. B., Szeminska, A., and Bang, V. (1977).Epistemology and the Psychology of Functions Reidel, Dordrecht.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piatelli-Palmerini, M. (1980).Language and Learning: The Debate Between Jean Piaget and Noam Chomsky Routledge & Kegan Paul, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S. (1984).Language Learnability and Language Development Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pomerantz, J. R. (1986). Visual form perception: An overview. In Schwab, E. C., and Nusbaum, H. C. (eds.),Pattern Recognition by Humans and Machines (Vol. 2): Visual Perception Academic Press, New York, pp. 1–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pressley, M., and Van Meter, P. (in press). Memory strategies: Natural development and use following instruction. In Pasnak, R., and Howe, M. (eds.),Emerging Themes in Cognitive Development (Vol. 2), Springer-Verlag, New York.

  • Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1985).Computation and Cognition MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reese, H. W. (1984).Advances in Child Development and Behavior (Vol. 18), Academic Press, Orlando.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reese, H. W., and Overton, W. F. (1970). Models of development and theories of development. In Goulet, L. R., and Baltes, P. B. (eds.),Life-Span Developmental Psychology: Research and Theory Academic Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Resnick, L. B. (1987). Constructing knowledge in school. In Liben, L. S. (ed.),Development and Learning: Conflict or Congruence? Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 19–50.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosch, E. (1978). Principles of categorization. In Rosch, E. and Lloyd, B. B. (eds.),Cognition and Categorization Lawrence Erbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ryle, G. (1971). Knowing how and knowing that. In Ryle, G. (ed.),Collected Papers (Vol. II), Barnes & Noble, New York, pp. 212–225.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schacter, D. L. (1989). Memory. In Posner, M. I. (ed.),Foundations of Cognitive Science MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, B. (1978).Psychology of Learning and Behavior W. W. Norton & Co., New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegler, R. S. (1976). Three aspects of cognitive development.Cogn. Psychol. 8: 481–520.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegler, R. S. (1983). Information processing approaches to cognitive development. In Mussen, P. H. (ed.),Handbook of Child Psychology (Vol. I), John Wiley & Sons, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegler, R. S., and Shrager, J. (1984). Strategy choices in addition and subtraction: How do children know what to do? In Sophian, C. (ed.),Origins of Cognitive Skills Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1974).About Behaviorism Alfred A. Knopf, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Staats, A. W. (1991). Unified positivism and unification psychology: Fad or new field?Am. Psychol. 46: 899–912.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thorndike, E. L. (1913/1970).Educational Psychology (Vol. 2), Greenwood Press, Westport, Conn.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. R. (1932).Purposive Behavior in Animals and Men Century, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tulving, E. (1985). How many memory systems are there?Am. Psychol. 40: 385–398.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1962).Thought and Language MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wartofsky, M. W. (1968).Conceptual Foundations of Scientific Thought Macmillan, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J. B. (1913). Psychology as the behaviorist views it.Psychol. Rev. 20: 158–177.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, H. M. (1988). First steps in the child's theorizing about the mind. In Astington, J., Harris, P., and Olson, D. (eds.),Developing Theories of Mind Cambridge University Press, New York, pp. 64–92.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winograd, T. (1975). Frame representations and the declarative/procedural controversy. In Bobrow, D. G. and Collins, A. (eds.),Representation and Understanding: Studies in Cognitive Science Academic Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • White, S. H. (1970). The learning theory approach. In Mussen, P. H. (eds.),Carmichael's Manual of Child Psychology (Vol. I), Weily, New York, pp. 657–701.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman, B. J., and Whitehurst, G. J. (1979). Structure and function: A comparison of two views of the development of language and cognition. In Whitehurst, G. J., and Zimmerman, B. J. (eds.),The Functions of Language and Cognition Academic Press, New York, pp. 1–23.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Byrnes, J.P. Categorizing and combining theories of cognitive development and learning. Educ Psychol Rev 4, 309–343 (1992). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01417875

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01417875

Key words

Navigation