Skip to main content
Log in

Landmark learning and visuo-spatial memories in gerbils

  • Published:
Journal of Comparative Physiology A Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Summary

  1. 1.

    The aim of this study is to understand what a rodent (Meriones unguiculatus) learns about the geometrical relations between a goal and nearby visual landmarks and how it uses this information to reach a goal. Gerbils were trained to find sunflower seeds on the floor of a light-tight, black painted room illuminated by a single light bulb hung from the ceiling. The position of the seed on the floor was specified by an array of one or more landmarks. Once training was complete, we recorded where the gerbils searched when landmarks were present but the seed was absent. In such tests, gerbils were confronted either with the array of landmarks to which they were accustomed or with a transformation of this array.

  2. 2.

    Animals searched in the appropriate spot when trained to find seeds placed in a constant direction and at a constant distance from a single cylindrical landmark (Fig. 1). Since gerbils look in one spot and not in a circle centred on the landmark, the direction between landmark and goal must be supplied by cues external to the landmark array. Distance, on the other hand, must be measured with respect to the landmark. Tests in which the size of the landmark was altered from that used in training suggest that distance is not learned solely in terms of the apparent size of the landmark as seen from the goal (Fig. 3).

  3. 3.

    Gerbils can still reach a goal defined by an array of landmarks when the room light is extinguished during their approach (Figs. 4, 5). This ability implies that they have already planned a trajectory to the goal before the room is darkened. In order to compute such a trajectory, their internal representation of landmarks and goal needs to contain information about the distances and bearings between landmarks and goal.

  4. 4.

    For planning trajectories, each landmark of an array can be used separately from the others (Fig. 7). Gerbils trained to a goal specified by an array of several landmarks were tested with one or more of the landmarks removed or with the array expanded. They then searched as though they had computed an independent trajectory for each landmark. For instance, gerbils trained with an array of two landmarks were tested with the distance between two landmarks doubled. The animals then searched for seeds in two positions, which were at the correct distance and in the right direction from each landmark.

  5. 5.

    If an internal representation of an array of landmarks is to be used to plan a trajectory, landmarks seen on the ground must be matched to those held in memory. One way in which gerbils do this is by learning properties of individual landmarks, such as their shape, size or surface markings (Figs. 10, 11, 13). For example, gerbils were able to locate seeds defined by a single relevant landmark while ignoring an irrelevant landmark with different features which was placed randomly with respect to the goal.

  6. 6.

    Several experiments (Figs. 4, 12, 13, 14) suggested that, although landmarks may be used independently for computing trajectories, the process of matching landmarks to the gerbil's representation requires a knowledge of the distances and directionsbetween landmarks.

  7. 7.

    We conclude that a gerbil's representation of its environment is complete in that it stores explicitly or can compute from what it has stored the geometric arrangement of landmarks and goal. We discuss the possibility that its spatial memories consist of a set of vectors describing the distance and direction from the goal to each landmark (Fig. 18) and consider the advantages and disadvantages of such a goal-centred memory.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Baker AG, Emerson VF (1983) Grating acuity of the mongolian gerbil. Behav Brain Res 8:195–208

    Google Scholar 

  • Balda RP, Turek RJ (1984) The cache-recovery system as an example of memory capabilities in Clark's nutcracker. In: Roitblat HL, Bever TG, Terrace HS (eds) Animal cognition. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale London, pp 513–532

    Google Scholar 

  • Cartwright BA, Collett TS (1983) Landmark learning in bees: experiments and models. J Comp Physiol 151:521–543

    Google Scholar 

  • Morris RGM (1981) Spatial localisation does not require the presence of local cues. Learn Motivation 12:239–260

    Google Scholar 

  • Naumov NP, Lobachev VS (1975) Ecology of desert rodents of the U.S.S.R. (Jerboas and gerbils). In: Prakash I, Ghosh PK (eds) Rodents in desert environments. Junk, The Hague, pp 465–598

    Google Scholar 

  • O'Keefe J, Nadel L (1978) The hippocampus as a cognitive map. Clarendon Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Olton DS (1982) Spatially organised behaviours of animals: behavioural and neurological studies. In: Potegal M (ed) Spatial abilities: Development and physiological foundations. Academic, New York London Paris San Diego San Francisco Sao Paulo Sydney Tokyo Toronto, pp 335–360

    Google Scholar 

  • Pick HL, Rieser JJ (1982) Children's cognitive mapping. In: Potegal M (ed) Spatial abilities: Development and physiological foundations. Academic, New York London Paris San Diego San Francisco Sao Paulo Sydney Toyko Toronto, pp 107–128

    Google Scholar 

  • Roberts WA (1984) Some issues in animal spatial memory. In: Roitblat HL, Bever TG, Terrace HS (eds) Animal cognition. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale London, pp 425–444

    Google Scholar 

  • Schöne H (1984) Spatial orientation — The spatial control of behaviour in animals and man. Princeton University, Princeton

    Google Scholar 

  • Sherry DF (1985) Food storage by birds and mammals. Adv Study Behav 15:153–188

    Google Scholar 

  • Suzuki S, Augerinos G, Black AH (1980) Stimulus control of spatial behaviour on the eight-arm maze in rats. Learn Motivation 11:1–18

    Google Scholar 

  • Thomson JA (1980) How do we use visual information to control locomotion. Trends Neurosci 3:247–250

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman EC, Ritchie BF, Kalish D (1946a) Studies in spatial learning. 1. Orientation and the short-cut. J Exp Psychol 36:13–24

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman EC, Ritchie BF, Kalish D (1946b) Studies in spatial learning. 11. Place learning versus response learning. J Exp Psychol 36:221–229

    Google Scholar 

  • Vander Wall SB (1982) An experimental analysis of cache recovery in Clark's nutcracker. Anim Behav 30:84–94

    Google Scholar 

  • Wehner R (1981) Spatial vision in arthropods. In: Autrum H (ed) Vision in invertebrates (Handbook of sensory physiology, vol V11/6C). Springer, Berlin Heidelberg New York, pp 287–617

    Google Scholar 

  • Wehner R, Räber F (1979) Visual spatial memory in desert ants,Cataglyphis bicolor (Hymenoptera: Formicidae). Experientia 35:1569–1571

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Collett, T.S., Cartwright, B.A. & Smith, B.A. Landmark learning and visuo-spatial memories in gerbils. J. Comp. Physiol. 158, 835–851 (1986). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01324825

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01324825

Keywords

Navigation