Skip to main content
Log in

How are questionnaire data similar to, and different from, thought-sampling data? Five studies manipulating retrospectiveness, single-moment focus, and indeterminacy

  • Published:
Cognitive Therapy and Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Results reported in the literature from thinking research are difficult to compare because the various studies differ on three independent dimensions: whether the observations are immediate or retrospective, whether the observations are targeted at measuring single moments in time or multiple moments, and whether the observations are directed at specific thought events or at characterizing thought events in general. Furthermore, questionnaire and sampling studies typically collect data on differing sets of variables. In an attempt to separate the influences of these dimensions in the thoughtsampling literature, factor-analytic techniques were applied to five studies in which subjects described their thoughts and moods on the same 42-variable rating questionnaire. Studies were designed to differentiate between factors produced using immediate versus retrospective techniques, single-moment versus multiple-moment techniques, and specific-event versus indeterminate-event techniques. Factors that had occurred in previous research called Aggressive/Bad Mood, Pleasant/Sexual, Clear Thought, Daydream/Past Sexual, Duration, and Self-Critical were found in all five studies. However, factor-loading patterns in the five studies were somewhat different, leading to the conclusions that the retrospective nature of questionnaires is likely to distort the frequency of recollections in thinking; that the indeterminate nature of retrospective questionnaires might lead to the erroneous conclusions that clarity in thinking is associated with pleasant thoughts and to a minimization in the reports of strength of negative affect; and that either the multiple-moment nature or the between-people analysis of retrospective questionnaires might lead to exaggeration of the relationship between sex and aggression in experience.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Antrobus, J. S., Singer, J. L., & Greenberg, S. (1966). Studies in the stream of consciousness: Experimental enhancement and suppression of spontaneous cognitive processes.Perceptual and Motor Skills, 23 399–417.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cameron, P., & Giuntoli, D. (1972). Consciousness sampling in the college classroom or Is anybody listening?Intellect. October, 63–64.

  • Cattell, R. B. (1957). Personality and motivation structure and measurement. New York: World Book.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M., Larson, R., & Prescott, S. (1977). The ecology of adolescent activity and experience.Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 6 281–294.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cundiff, G., & Gold, S. R. (1979). Daydreaming: A measurable concept.Perceptual and Motor Skills, 49 347–353.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Giambra, L. M. (1974). Daydreaming across the lifespan: Late adolescence to senior citizen.Aging and Human Development, 5 116–135.

    Google Scholar 

  • Giambra, L. M. (1977). Daydreaming about the past—The time setting of spontaneous thought intrusions.Gerontologist, 17 35–38.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Giambra, L. M. (1981). Daydreaming, attentional processes, and curiosity in white Americans: Religious, educational, economic and residency influences for a life span sample.Journal of Clinical Psychology, 37 262–275.

    Google Scholar 

  • Giambra, L. M. (1983). Daydreaming in 40- to 60-year old women: Menopause, health, values, and sexuality.Journal of Clinical Psychology, 39 11–21.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Giambra, L. M., & Traynor, T. D. (1978). Depression and daydreaming: An analysis based on self-ratings.Journal of Clinical Psychology, 34 14–25.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gilchrist, M. B. (1982). Creative talent and academic competence.Genetic Psychology Monographs, 106 261–318.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gold, S. R., & Cundiff, G. (1980). Increasing the frequency of daydreaming.Journal of Clinical Psychology, 36 116–121.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huba, G. J., Segal, B., & Singer, J. L. (1977). Consistency of daydreaming styles across samples of college male and female drug and alcohol users.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 86 99–102.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Humphrey, G. (1951).Thinking. London: Methuen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hurlburt, R. T. (1976). Self-observation and self-control. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of South Dakota.

  • Hurlburt, R. T. (1979). Random sampling of cognition and behavior.Journal of Research in Personality, 13 103–111.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hurlburt, R. T. (1980). Validation and correlation of thought sampling with retrospective measures.Cognitive Therapy and Research, 4 235–238.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hurlburt, R. T., Lech, B. C., & Saltman, S. (1984). Random sampling of thought and mood.Cognitive Therapy and Research, 8 263–275.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hurlburt, R. T., & Melancon, S. M. (1987a). P-technique factor analyses of individuals' thought and mood-sampling data.Cognitive Therapy and Research, 11 487–500.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hurlburt, R. T., & Melancon, S. M. (1987b). Correlates of thought-sampling data in a classroom setting. In press.Journal of Research in Personality.

  • Joffe, C. H. (Producer). (1977).Annie Hall. New York: United Artists. (Film).

    Google Scholar 

  • Klinger, E. (1978–1979). Dimensions of thought and imagery in normal waking states.Journal of Altered States of Consciousness, 4 97–103.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klinger, E., Barta, S. G., & Maxeiner, M. E. (1980). Motivational correlates of thought content frequency and commitment.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39 1222–1237.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klinger, E., & Cox, W. M. (1985).Dimensions of thought flow in everyday life. Paper presented at the American Psychological Association Annual Meeting, Los Angeles.

  • Pope, K. H., & Singer, J. L. (Eds.). (1978).The stream of consciousness: Scientific investigations into the flow of human experience. New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Segal, B., & Singer, J. L. (1976). Daydreaming, drug, and alcohol use in college students: A factor analytic study.Additive Behaviors, 1 227–235.

    Google Scholar 

  • Singer, J. L. (1974). Daydreaming and the stream of thought.American Scientist, 2 417–425.

    Google Scholar 

  • Singer, J. L. (1975a).The inner world of daydreaming. Harper & Row.

  • Singer, J. L. (1975b). Navigating the stream of consciousness: Research in daydreaming and related inner experience.American Psychologist, 30 727–738.

    Google Scholar 

  • Singer, J. L., & Antrobus, J. S. (1970).Imaginal processes inventory. Princeton: Educational Testing Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Starker, S. (1978). Dreams and waking fantasy. In K. S. Pope & J. L. Singer (Eds.),The stream of consciousness: Scientific investigation into the flow of human experience (pp. 302–319). New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Starker, S., & Singer, J. L. (1975). Daydreaming and symptom patterns of psychiatric patients: A factor analytic study.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 84 567–570.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Hurlburt, R.T., Melancon, S.M. How are questionnaire data similar to, and different from, thought-sampling data? Five studies manipulating retrospectiveness, single-moment focus, and indeterminacy. Cogn Ther Res 11, 681–703 (1987). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01176005

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01176005

Key words

Navigation