Skip to main content
Log in

The effects of increasing the number of stimulus modalities included in a fear-eliciting imagery scene on reported imagery clarity, scene repetition, and sympathetic (fear) arousal

  • Published:
Cognitive Therapy and Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The present study tested the hypothesis that autonomic responding to imagery instructions can be enhanced by increasing the number of stimulus modalities associated with the feared stimulus. This prediction was supported by the skin-conductance frequency data. The theory under consideration also resulted in a prediction that imagery ratings of clarity would initially be lower as the number of modalities (stimulus intensity of the scenes) increased. It was further predicted that with scene repetition and subsequent fear extinction, imagery clarity would then increase. The scenes were presented in the form of stimulus propositions. The data supported both of these predictions. An additional hypothesis that the autonomic differences between subjects who rated themselves prior to the experiment as either low or high in fear to the target stimulus would diminish as a direct function of an increase in the number of stimulus modalities presented was only partially supported. The paper presents the underlying rationale for these predictions as well as noting the clinical relevance of the findings.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Anderson, M. P., & Borkovec, T. D. (1980). Imagery processing and fear reduction during repeated exposure to two types of phobic imagery.Behaviour Research and Therapy, 18 537.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bauer, R., & Craighead, W. E. (1979). Psychophysiological responses to the imagination of fearful and neutral situations: The effects of imagery instructions.Behavior Therapy, 10 389–403.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carroll, D., Marzillier, J. S., & Merian, S. (1982). Psychophysiological changes accompanying different types of arousing and relaxing imagery.Psychophysiology, 19 75–82.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carroll, D., Marzillier, J. S., & Watson, F. (1980). Heart rate and self-report changes accompanying different types of relaxing imagery.Behaviour Research and Therapy, 18 273–280.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dollard, J., & Miller, N. E. (1950).Personality and psychotherapy. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Edelberg, R. (1967). Electrical properties of the skin. In C. Brown (Ed.),Methods in psychophysiology. Baltimore: Williams and Wilkins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elliott, R. (1974). The motivational significance of heart rate. In P. A. Obrist, A. H. Black, J. Brener, & L. V. DiCara (Eds.),Cardiovascular psychophysiology: Current issues in response mechanisms, biofeedback and methodology. Chicago: Aldine.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foa, E. B., & Kozak, M. J. (1986). Emotional processing of fear: Exposure to corrective information.Psychological Bulletin, 99 20–35.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Geer, J. H. (1965). The development of a scale to measure fear.Behaviour Research and Therapy, 3 45–53.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaloupek, D. G., & Levis, D. J. (1980). The relationship between stimulus specificity and selfreport indices in assessing fear of heterosexual social interaction: A test of the unitary response hypothesis.Behavioral Assessment, 2 267–281.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaloupek, D. G., Peterson, P. A., Boyd, T. L., & Levis, D. J. (1981). The effects of exposure to a spatial ordered fear stimulus: A study of generalization of extinction effects.Behavior Therapy, 12 130–137.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lacey, J. I. (1959). Psychophysiological approaches to the evaluation of psychotherapeutic process and outcome. In E. A. Rubenstein & M. B. Parloff (Eds.),Research in psychotherapy (Vol. 1). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lacey, J. I., & Lacey, B. C. (1970). Some autonomic-central nervous system interrelationships. In P. Black (Ed.),Physiological correlates of emotion. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lang, P. J. (1977). Imagery in therapy: An information processing analysis of fear.Behavior Therapy, 8 862–886.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lang, P. J., Kozak, M. J., Miller, G. A., Levin, D. N., & McLean, A., Jr. (1980). Emotional imagery: Conceptual structure and pattern of somato-visceral response.Psychophysiology, 17 179–192.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lang, P. J., Levin, D. N., Miller, G. A., & Kozak, M. J. (1983). Fear behavior, fear imagery, and the psychophysiology of emotion: The problem of affective response integration.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 92 276–306.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lang, P. J., Melamed, B. G., & Hart, J. (1970). A psychophysiological analysis of fear modification using an automated desensitization procedure.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 76 229–234.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levis, D. J. (1969). The phobic test apparatus: An objective measure of human avoidance behavior to small objects.Behaviour Research and Therapy, 7 309–315.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Levis, D. J. (1980). Implementing the technique of implosive therapy. In A. Goldstein & E. B. Foa (Eds.),Handbook of behavioral interventions. A clinical guide (pp. 92–151). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levis, D. J. (1985). Implosive theory: A comprehensive extention of conditioning theory of fear/anxiety to psychopathology. In S. Reiss & R. R. Bootzin (Eds.),Theoretical issues in behavior therapy. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lick, J. R., Sushinsky, L. W., & Malow, R. (1977). Specificity of fear survey schedule items and the prediction of avoidance behavior.Behavior Modification, 1 195–204.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lykken, D. T., & Venables, P. (1971). Direct measurement of skin conductance: A proposal for standardization.Psychophysiology, 8 565–672.

    Google Scholar 

  • May, J. R. (1977). Psychophysiology of self-regulated phobic thoughts.Behavior Therapy, 8 150–159.

    Google Scholar 

  • McReynolds, W. T., & Stegman, R. (1976). Sayer versus sign.Behavior Therapy, 7 704–705.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mowrer, O. H. (1960).Learning theory and the symbolic processes. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Obrist, P. A. (1981). Cardiovascular psychophysiology—A perspective. New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Obrist, P. A. (1983). Cardiac-behavioral interactions: A critical appraisal. In J. T. Cacioppo & R. E. Petty (Eds.),Perspectives in cardiovascular psychophysiology. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paivio, A. (1971).Imagery and verbal processes. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, D. A., & Levis, D. J. (1985). The assessment of bodily injury fears via the behavioral avoidance slide-test.Behavioral Assessment, 7 173–184.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rescorla, R. A., & Solomon, R. L. (1967). Two-process learning theory: Relationships between Pavlovian conditioning and instrumental learning.Psychological Review, 74 151–182.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, A., & Reading, C. (1985). Imagery in phobic subjects: A psychophysiological study.Behaviour Research and Therapy, 23 247–254.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sheehan, P. W. (1972).The function and nature of imagery. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stampfl, T. G. (1970). Implosive therapy: An emphasis on covert stimulation. In D. J. Levis (Ed.),Learning approaches to therapeutic behavior change. Chicago: Aldine.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stampfl, T. G., & Levis, D. J. (1987). The essentials of implosive therapy: A learning-theorybased psychodynamic behavioral therapy.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 72 496–503.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tecce, J. J., & Cole, J. D. (1972). Psychophysiological responses of schizophrenics to drugs.Psychopharmacologia, 24 159–200.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van Egeren, L. F. Feather, B. W., & Hern, P. L. (1971). Desensitization of phobia: Some psychophysiological propositions.Psychophysiology, 8 213–228.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vrana, S. R., Cuthbert, B. N., Lang, P. J. (1986). Fear imagery and text processing.Psychophysiology, 23 247–253.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weerts, T. C., & Lang, P. J. (1978). The psychophysiology of fear imagery: Difference between focal phobia and social-performance anxiety.Journal of Constulting and Clinical Psychology, 46 1157–1159.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolpe, J. (1958).Psychotherapy by Reciprocal Inhibition. Stanford: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Holzman, A.D., Levis, D.J. The effects of increasing the number of stimulus modalities included in a fear-eliciting imagery scene on reported imagery clarity, scene repetition, and sympathetic (fear) arousal. Cogn Ther Res 13, 389–405 (1989). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01173481

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01173481

Key words

Navigation