Skip to main content
Log in

A comparison of hyponym and synonym decisions

  • Published:
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Is class inclusion (hyponymy) a more primitive or simpler semantic relation than synonymy? This question was addressed by comparing the time required to identify examples of the two relations in a semantic decision task. In two experiments subjects made truelfalse decisions about statements of the form “An A is a B.” In Experiment 1 category-member and synonym pairs were randomly intermixed; there was no difference between the two relations. In Experiment 2 one group was presented with the two relations randomly intermixed, as in Experiment 1 (mixed condition), while two other groups were each presented with just one of the relations (separate condition). In the separate condition responses were faster to class inclusion than to synonym pairs, while in the mixed condition there was no difference, as in Experiment 1. The results suggest that class inclusion may be a simpler relation than synonymity, although the difference may simply reflect the use to which the two relations are put in common use. The fact that the difference occurred in the separate but not in the mixed conditions suggests that the latencies reflected the evaluation of the relations against a decision criterion rather than directly reflecting lexical organization or everyday usage.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Anderson, J. R. (1976):Language, memory, and thought. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beckwith, R., Fellbaum, C., Gross, D., & Miller, G. A. (1990).WordNet: A lexical database organized on psycholinguistic principles, Princeton, NJ: Princeton University, Cognitive Science Laboratory, Report No. 42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bolinger, D. (1965). The atomization of meaning.Language, 41, 555–573.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chaffin, R. (1981). Context effects in the categorization task.Journal of General Psychology, 104, 293–302.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chaffin, R., & Herrmann, D. J. (1987). Relation element theory: A new account of the representation and processing of semantic relations. In D. Gorfein & R. Hoffman (Eds.)Learning and memory: The Ebbingaus Centennial Conference. (pp. 221–245). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chaffin, R., & Herrmann, D. J. (1988). Effects of relation similarity on part-whole decisions.Journal of General Psychology, 115, 131–139.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chaffin, R., Herrmann, D. J., & Winston, M. E. (1988). An empirical taxonomy of part-whole relations: Effects of part-whole relation type on relation identification.Language and Cognitive Processes, 3, 17–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chang, T. M. (1986). Semantic memory: Facts and models.Psychological Bulletin, 99, 199–220.

    Google Scholar 

  • Collins, A. M., & Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading activation theory of semantic processing.Psychological Review, 82, 407–428.

    Google Scholar 

  • Conrad, C. (1972). Cognitive economy in semantic memory.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 92, 149–154.

    Google Scholar 

  • Forster, K. I., & Dickinson, R. G. (1976). More on the language as fixed effect fallacy: Monte Carlo estimates of error rates forF 1,F 2, andmin F'.Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 15, 135–142.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glass, A. L., & Holyoak, K. J. (1975). Alternative conceptions of semantic memory.Cognition, 3, 313–319.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gross, D., Fischer, U., & Miller, G. A. (1989). The organization of adjectival meaning.Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 92–106.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gruenenfelder, T. M. (1986). Relational similarity and context effects in category verification.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 12, 587–599.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herrmann, D. J. (1978). An old problem for the new psychosemantics: Synonymity.Psychological Bulletin, 85, 490–512.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herrmann, D. J., & Chaffin, R. (1986). Comprehension of semantic relations as a function of definitions of relations. In F. Klix (Ed.)Learning and memory. (pp. 311–319). Berlin: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herrmann, D. J., Chaffin, R., Conti, G., Peters, D., & Robbins, P. H. (1979). Comprehension of antonymy: The generality of categorization models.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 5, 585–597.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herrmann, D. J., Chaffin, R., Daniel, M. P., & Wool, R. S. (1986). The role of elements of relation definitions in antonym and synonym comprehension.Zeitschrift fur Psychologie, 194, 134–153.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herrmann, D. J., Chaffin, R., & Winston, M. E. (1986). “Robins are a part of birds”: The confusion of semantic relations.Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 24, 413–415.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holyoak, K. J., & Glass, A. L. (1975). The role of contradictions and counterexamples in the rejection of false sentences.Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 14, 215–239.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hume, D. (1965).A treatise of human nature. L. A. Selby Bigge (Ed.). Oxford: Clarendon Press. (Original published 1739).

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson-Laird, P. N., Herrmann, D. J., & Chaffin, R. (1984). Only connections: A critique of semantic networks.Psychological Bulletin, 96, 292–315.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klix, F., & van der Meer, E. (1980). The method of analogy recognition for the determination of semantic relations in long-term memory. In F. Klix & J. Hoffmann (Eds.)Cognition and memory. (pp. 145–152). Berlin: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kucera, H., & Francis, W. N. (1967).Computational analysis of present-day American English. Providence, RI: Brown University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kunzendorf, R. G. (1976). Selected features of word meaning.Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 15, 633–640.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lorch, R. F., Jr. (1981). Effects of relation strength and semantic overlap on retrieval and comparison processes during sentence verification.Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 20, 593–610.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lyons, J. (1977).Semantics. Volume I. London: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCloskey, M., & Glucksberg, S. (1979). Decision processes in verifying category membership statements: Implications for models of semantic memory.Cognitive Psychology, 11, 1–37.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. A. (1990). Inheritance systems (unpublished manuscript).

  • Miller, G. A., & Charles, W. (1990). Contextual correlates of semantic similarity (in preparation).

  • Miller, G. A., & Fellbaum, C. (1990). Semantic networks of English.Cognition (in press).

  • Miller, G. A., & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1976).Language and perception, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norman, D. A., Rumelhart, D. E., & the LNR Group (1975).Explorations in cognition. San Francisco: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Poltrock, S. E., & Foltz, G. S. (1982). An experimental psychology laboratory system for the Apple II microcomputer.Behavior Research Methods & Instrumentation, 14, 103–108.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schank, R. C. (1972). Conceptual dependence: A theory of natural language understanding.Cognitive Psychology, 3, 552–631.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, E. E., Shoben, E. J., & Ripps, L. J. (1974). Structure and process in semantic memory: A featural model of semantic decisions.Psychological Review, 81, 214–241.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whitten, II, W. B., Suter, N., & Frank, M. L. (1979). Bidirectional synonym ratings of 464 noun pairs.Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 18, 109–127.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilkins, A. J. (1971). Conjoint frequency, category size, and categorization time.Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 10, 382–885.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winston, M. E., Chaffin, R., & Herrmann, D. J. (1987). A taxonomy of part-whole relations.Cognitive Science, 11, 417–444.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Chaffin, R., Glass, A. A comparison of hyponym and synonym decisions. J Psycholinguist Res 19, 265–280 (1990). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01077260

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01077260

Keywords

Navigation