Skip to main content
Log in

The current status of two sublines of the Roman High and Low Avoidance strains

  • Published:
Behavior Genetics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Experiments are described which assess the behavioral status of two sublines of the Roman High and Low Avoidance strains which have been maintained by brother x sister mating without selection at the Institute of Psychiatry, University of London. The strains demonstrate significant differences in the avoidance behavior for which they were originally selected. However, the Low Avoiding strain did show some high-scoring individuals and a degree of selection pressure was consequently applied to this line. The results of this selection and the performance of these strains in an open field are reported. The data are discussed in the light of a recent report that other sublines no longer display differences in avoidance conditioning.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bättig, K., Driscoll, P., Schlatter, J., and Uster, H. J. (1976). Effects of nicotine on the exploratory locomotion patterns of female Roman high-and low-avoidance rats.Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 4:435–439.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bignami, G. (1965). Selection for high rates and low rates of avoidance conditioning in the rat.Animal Behav. 13:221–227.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broadhurst, P. L., and Bignami, G. (1965). Correlative effects of psychogenetic selection: A study of the Roman high and low avoidance strains of rats.Behav. Res. Ther. 2:273–280.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coyle, J. T., Wender, P., and Lipsky, A. (1974). Avoidance conditioning in different strains of rat: Neurochemical correlates.Psychopharmacologia 31:25–34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Driscoll, P., and Bättig, K. (1982). Behavioral, emotional and neurochemical profiles of rats selected for extreme differences in active, two-way avoidance performance. In Lieblich, I. (ed.),Genetics of the Brain, Elesevier/North-Holland, Amsterdam, pp. 95–123.

    Google Scholar 

  • Durcan, M. J., Fulker, D. W., and Campbell, I. C. (1984). Differences in the stereotypy response but not the hypomotility response to apomorphine in the Roman High and Low Avoiding strains of rats.Psychopharmacology 82:215–220.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fulker, D. W., Wilcock, J., and Broadhurst, P. L. (1972). Studies in genotype-environment interaction. I. Methodology and preliminary multivariate analysis of a diallel cross of eight strains of rat.Behav. Genet. 2:261–287.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gentsch, C., Lichtensteiner, M., Driscoll, P., and Freer, H. (1982). Differential hormonal and physiological responses to stress in Roman high-and low-avoidance rats.Physiol. Behav. 28:259–263.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hewitt, J. K., and Fulker, D. W. (1983). Using the triple test cross to investigate the genetics of behavior in wild populations. II. Escape-avoidance conditioning inRattus norvegicus.Behav. Genet. 13:1–5.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hewitt, J. K., Fulker, D. W., and Broadhurst, P. L. (1981). Genetics of escape-avoidance conditioning in laboratory and wild populations of rats.Behav. Genet. 11:533–544.

    Google Scholar 

  • Imada, H. (1972). Emotional reactivity and conditionability in four strains of rats.J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 79:474–480.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, J. R., Overstreet, D. H., Driscoll, P., and Bättig, K. (1981). Effects of scopolamine, pilocarpine and oxotremorine on the exploratory behavior of two psychogentically selected lines of rats in a complex maze.Psychopharmacology 72:135–142.

    Google Scholar 

  • Satinder, K. P. (1971). Genotype-dependant effects of d-amphetamine sulphate and caffeine on escape-avoidance behavior of rats.J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 76:359–364.

    Google Scholar 

  • Satinder, K. P. (1980). Genetically heterogenous and selected lines of rats: Behavioral and reproductive comparison.Behav. Genet. 10:191–200.

    Google Scholar 

  • van der Staay, F. J., Raaijmakers, W., and Kerbush, S. (1983). Rapid acquisition of two-way active avoidance in inbred Roman Low Avoidance rats.Behav. Genet. 13:435–440.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilcock, J., and Fulker, D. W. (1973). Avoidance learning in rats: Genetic evidence for two distinct behavioral processes in the shuttle-box.J. Comp. Physiol. Psyychol. 82:247–253.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilcock, J., Fulker, D. W., and Broadhurst, P. L. (1981). Analysis of two-way escapeavoidance conditioning measures from a diallel cross of eight strains of rats.Behav. Genet. 11:339–358.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

This work was supported by Grant RF7038 from the Bethlem/Maudsley Research Fund.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Durcan, M.J., Wraight, K.B. & Fulker, D.W. The current status of two sublines of the Roman High and Low Avoidance strains. Behav Genet 14, 559–569 (1984). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01068126

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01068126

Key Words

Navigation