Skip to main content
Log in

Reading inflectionally incogruent texts: The automatic and strategic processes in language performance

  • Published:
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Subjects were asked to read out loud at the fastest possible speed texts containing incorrectly inflected verbs exactly as the texts were printed. A control condition (errorfree texts) and two error conditions were used, one with half of the verbs (50%) containing errors, and the other with all the verbs (100%) containing error. Compared with reading correct texts, reading in the 50% condition was slower and more frequently impeded by different kinds of perturbations. This lowered performance was considered to have resulted from interference produced by covert involuntary activation of prior linguistic associations between the contexts and the appropriate verb forms. Performance in the 100% condition improved over the 50% condition. The improvement suggests that a strategic process had been induced to deal with the task demands. The main theoretical point is that complex mental processes such as language performance are automatic, and yet not rigid, unlike what the classic view of automaticity would predict.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bock, J. K. (1982). Towards a cognitive psychology of syntax: Information processing contributions to sentence formulation.Psychological Review, 89, 1–47.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bock, J. K. (1986). Syntactic persistence in language production.Cognitive Psychology, 18, 355–387.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butterworth, B. (1980). Evidence from pauses in speech. In B. Butterworth (Ed.),Language production, Vol 1: Speech and talk (pp. 154–176). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crain, S., & Fodor, J. D. (1987). Sentence matching and overgeneration.Cognition, 26, 123–169.

    Google Scholar 

  • den Heyer, K. (1986). Manipulating attention-induced priming in a lexical decision task by means of repeated prime-target presentation.Journal of Memory and Language, 25, 19–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dyer, F. N. (1973). The Stroop phenomenon and its use in the study of perceptual, cognitive, and response process.Memory and Cognition, 1, 106–120.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flores d'Arcais, G. B. (1982). Automatic syntactic computation and use of semantic information during sentence comprehension.Psychological Research, 44, 231–242.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flores d'Arcais, G. B. (1987). Syntactic processing during reading for comprehension. In M. Coltheart (Ed.),Attention and performance XII: The psychology of reading (pp. 619–633). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Forster, K. I. (1981). Priming and the effect of sentence and lexical contexts on naming time: Evidence for autonomous lexical processing.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, A, 33, 465–495.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frazier, L., Taft, L., Roeper, T., Clifton, C., & Ehrlich, K. (1984). Parallel structure: A source of facilitation in sentence comprehension.Memory and Cognition, 12, 421–430.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodman, G. O., McClelland, J. L., & Gibbs, Jr., R. W. (1981). The role of syntactic context in word recognition.Memory and Cognition, 9, 580–586.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graesser, A. C., Hoffman, N. L., & Clark, L. F. (1980). Structural components of reading time.Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 135–151.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graesser, A. C., & Riha, J. R. (1984). An application of multiple regression techniques to sentence reading times. In D. E. Kieras & M. A. Just (Eds.),New methods in reading comprehension research (pp. 183–218). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. T. (1977). Automatic and effortful processes in memory.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 108, 356–388.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. T. (1984). Automatic processing of fundamental information.American Psychologist, 39, 1372–1388.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jou, J., & Harris, R. J. (1991). Processing inflectons: Dynamic processes in sentence comprehension.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 1082–1094.

    Google Scholar 

  • Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). A theory of reading: From eye fixations to comprehension.Psychological Review, 87, 329–354.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., & Treisman, A. (1984). Changing views of attention and automaticity. In R. Parasuraman, R. Davies, & J. Beatty (Eds.),Varieties of attention. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Katz, L., Boyce, S., Goldstein, L., & Lukatela, G. (1987). Grammatical information effects in auditory word recognition.Cognition, 25, 235–263.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S. A. (1983). Event-related brain potentials to grammatical errors and semantic anomalies.Memory and Cognition, 11, 539–550.

    Google Scholar 

  • LaBerge, D., & Samuels, S. J. (1974). Toward a theory of automatic information processing in reading.Cognitive Psychology, 6, 293–323.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levelt, W. J. M. (1983). Monitoring and self-repair in speech.Cognition, 14, 41–104.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levelt, W. J. M. (1989).Speaking: From intention to articulation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Logan, G. D. (1985). Skill and automaticity: Relations, implications, and future directions.Canadian Journal of Psychology, 39(2), 367–386.

    Google Scholar 

  • Logan, G. D., & Zbrodoff, N. J. (1979). When it helps to be misled: Facilitative effects of increasing the frequency of conflicting stimuli in a Stroop-like task.Memory and Cognition, 7, 166–174.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lukatela, G., Kostic, A., Feldman, L. B., & Turvey, M. T. (1983). Grammatical priming of inflected nouns.Memory and Cognition, 11, 59–63.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, C. M., & Dunbar, K. (1988). Training and Stroop-like interference: Evidence for a continuum of automaticity.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 14, 126–135.

    Google Scholar 

  • Masson, M. E. J., & Freedman, L. (1990). Fluent identification of repeated words.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 355–373.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neely, J. H. (1977). Semantic priming and retrieval from lexical memory: Roles of inhibitionless spreading activation and limited-capacity attention.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 106, 226–254.

    Google Scholar 

  • Palmer, J., & Jonides, J. (1988). Automatic memory search and the effect of information load and irrelevant information.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 14, 136–144.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I., & Snyder, C. R. R. (1975). Attention and cognitive control. In R. L. Solso (Ed.),Information processing and cognition: The Loyola Symposium (pp. 55–85). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I., Snyder, C. R. R., & Davidson, B. J. (1980). Attention and the detection of signals.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 109, 160–174.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prinzmental, W., Hoffman, H., & Vest, K. (1991). Automatic processes in word perception: An analysis from illusory conjunction.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 17, 902–923.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salasoo, A., Shiffrin, R. M., & Feustel, T. C. (1985). Building permanent memory codes: Codification and repetition effects in word identification.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 114, 50–77.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W., Dumais, S. T., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1984). Automatic and control processing and attention. In R. Parasuraman & D. R. Davies (Eds.),Varieties of attention (pp. 1–27). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: I. Detection, search, and attention.Psychological Review, 84, 1–66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shiffrin, R. M., & Schneider, W. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending, and a general theory.Psychological Review, 84, 127–190.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (1983). On priming by a sentence context.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 112, 1–36.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reaction.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–662.

    Google Scholar 

  • West, R. F., & Stanovich, K. E. (1986). Robust effects of syntactic structure on visual word processing.Memory and Cognition, 14, 104–112.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wright, B., & Garrett, M. (1984). Lexical decision in sentences: Effects of syntactic structure.Memory and Cognition, 12, 31–45.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zacks, R. T., hasher, L., & Sanft, H. (1982). Automatic encoding of event frequency: Further findings.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 8, 106–116.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zbrodoff, N. J., & Logan, G. D. (1986). On the autonomy of mental processes: A case study of arithmetic.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 115, 118–130.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Jou, J. Reading inflectionally incogruent texts: The automatic and strategic processes in language performance. J Psycholinguist Res 21, 365–382 (1992). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01067921

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01067921

Keywords

Navigation