Skip to main content
Log in

Processing a dynamic visual—Spatial language: Psycholinguistic studies of American Sign Language

  • Published:
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

American Sign Language (ASL) has evolved within a completely different biological medium, using the hands and face rather than the vocal tract and perceived by eye rather than by ear. The research reviewed in this article addresses the consequences of this different modality for language processing, linguistic structure, and spatial cognition. Language modality appears to affect aspects of lexical recognition and the nature of the grammatical form used for reference. Select aspects of nonlinguistic spatial cognition (visual imagery and face discrimination) appear to be enhanced in deaf and hearing ASL signers. It is hypothesized that this enhancement is due to experience with a visual-spatial language and is tied to specific linguistic processing requirements (interpretation of grammatical facial expression, perspective transformations, and the use of topographic classifiers). In addition, adult deaf signers differ in the age at which they were first exposed to ASL during childhood. The effect of late acquisition of language on linguistic processing is investigated in several studies. The results show selective effects of late exposure to ASL on language processing, independent of grammatical knowledge.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bellugi, U., & Emmorey, K. (1993). Enhanced spatial abilities in adult deaf signers.

  • Bellugi, U., O'Grady, L., Lillo-Martin, D., O'Grady, M., van Hoek, K., & Corina, D. (1990). Enhancement of spatial cognition in deaf children. In V. Volterra & C. Erting (Eds.),From gesture to language in hearing and deaf children (pp. 278–298). New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bellugi, U., Poizner, H., & Kilma, E. S. (1990). Mapping brain function for language: Evidence from sign language. In G. M. Edelman, W. E. Gall, & W. M. Cowan (Eds.),Signal and senses: Local and global order in perceptual maps. (pp. 521–543). New York: Wiley-Liss.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bettger, J. (1992).The effects of experience on spatial cognition: Deafness and knowledge of ASL. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bybee, J. (1985).Morphology, Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Comrie, B. (1976).Aspect. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corina, D. P., Bellugi, U., Kritchevsky, M., O'Grady-Batch, L., & Norman, F. (1990).Spatial relations in signed versus spoken language: Clues to right parietal functions.

  • Corina, D. P., Emmorey, K. (1993). Phonological and Semantic Priming in ASL.

  • Corina, D. P., Kritchevsky, M., & Bellugi, U. (1992). Linguistic permeability of unilateral neglect: Evidence from American sign language.Proceedings of the 14th annual conference of the cognitive science society. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cutler, A., Mehler, J., Norris, D., & Segui, J. (1986). The syllable's role in the segmentation of French and English.Memory and Language.25(4), 385–400.

    Google Scholar 

  • Emmorey, K. (1991). Repetition priming with aspect and agreement morphology in American Sign Language.Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 20(5), 365–388.

    Google Scholar 

  • Emmorey, K. (1992).Processing topographic vs. arbitrary space in ASL. Paper presented at the International conference on theoretical issues in sign language research, San Diego, CA.

  • Emmorey, K., Bellugi, U., Friederici, A., & Horn, P. (1992).Effects of age of acquisition on grammatical sensitivity: Evidence from on-line and off-line tasks. Manuscript submitted for publication.

  • Emmorey, K., & Corina, D. (1990). Lexical recognition in sign language: Effects of phonetic structure and morphology.Perceptual and Motor Skills, 71, 1227–1252.

    Google Scholar 

  • Emmorey, K., & Corina, D. (1992).Differential sensitivity to classifier morphology in ASL signers. Paper presented at the Linguistic Society of America, Chicago, IL.

  • Emmorey, K., Kosslyn, S. M., & Bellugi, U. (1993). Visual imagery and visual-spatial language: Enhanced imagery ability in deaf and hearing ASL signers.Cognition, 46, 139–181.

    Google Scholar 

  • Emmorey, K., & Lillo-Martin, D. (1991).Processing spatial anaphora: Referent activation from overt and null pronouns in ASL. Paper presented at CUNY Sentence Processing Conference, Rochester, NY.

  • Emmorey, K., Norman, F., & O'Grady, L. (1991). The activation of spatial antecedents from overt pronouns in American Sign Language.Language and Cognitive Processes, 6(3), 207–228.

    Google Scholar 

  • Farah, M. (1988). Is visual imagery really visual? Overlooked evidence from neuropsychology.Psychological Review, 95(3), 307–317.

    Google Scholar 

  • Finke, R. A., & Shepard, R. N. (1986). Visual functions of mental imagery. In K. R. Boff, L. Kaufman, & J. P. Thomas (Eds.),Handbook of perception and human performance. New York: Wiley-Interscience.

    Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, L. A. (1975). Space, time, and person reference in American sign language.Language, 51, 940–961.

    Google Scholar 

  • Galvan, D. (1989). A sensitive period for the acquisition of complex morphology: Evidence from American Sign Language.Papers and Reports on Child Language Development, 28.

  • Grosjean, F. (1980). Spoken word recognition processes and the gating paradigm.Perception and Psychophysics, 28, 267–283.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grosjean, F. (1981). Sign and word recognition: A first comparison.Sign Language Studies, 32, 195–219.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klima, E., & Bellugi, U. (1979).The signs of language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University, Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kosslyn, S. M. (1980)Image and mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kosslyn, S. M. Brunn, J. L., Cave, K. R., & Wallach, R. W. (1985) Individual differences in mental imagery ability: A computational analysis.Cognition, 18, 195–243.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kosslyn, S., Cave, C., Provost, D., & Von Gierke, S. (1988). Sequential processes in image generation.Cognitive Psychology, 20, 319–343.

    Google Scholar 

  • Liddell, S. (1980).American Sign Language syntax. The Hague: Mouton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Liddell, S. (1990). Four functions of a locus: Re-examining the structure of space in ASL. In C. Lucas (Ed.),Sign language research, theoretical issues (pp. 176–198). Washington, DC: Gallaudet College Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lillo-Martin, D., & Klima, E. (1990). Pointing out differences: ASL pronouns in syntactic theory. In S. D. Fischer, & P. Siple, (Eds.),Theoretical issues in sign language research, (Vol. 1, pp. 191–210). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lillo-Martin, D. (1991).Universal grammar and American Sign Language: Setting the null argument parameters. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marslen-Wilson, W. (1987). Functional parallelism in spoken work recognition.Cognition, 25, 71–102.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marslen-Wilson, W., & Tyler, L. K. (1981). Central processes in speech understanding.Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, B, 295, 317–332.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayberry, R. (1992).Mental phonology in sign language. Paper presented at Theoretical Issues in Sign Language Research University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, CA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayberry, R., & Eichen, E. (1991). The long-lasting advantage of learning sign language in childhood: Another look at the critical period for language acquisition.Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 486–512.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayberry, R., & Fischer, S. (1989) Looking through phonological shape to sentence meaning: The bottleneck of non-native sign language processing.Memory and Cognition, 17, 740–754.

    Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, J. L., & Elman, J. L. (1986). A distributed model of human learning and memory. In D. E. Rumelhart, J. L. McClelland, & the PDP Research Group (Eds.),Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the micrsctructure of cognition (Vol. 2). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meier, R. (1981). Icons and morphemes: Models of the acquisition of verb agreement in ASL.Papers and Reports on Child Language Development, 20, 92–99.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newport, E. (1990). Maturational constrains on language learning.Cognitive Science, 14, 11–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newport, E. (1991). Contrasting conceptions of the critical period for language. In S. Carey and R. Gelman (Eds),The epigenesis of mind: Essays in biology and cognition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perlmutter, D. (1988).A moraic theory of ASL syllable structure. Paper presented at Theoretical Issues in Sign Language Research, Gallaudet University, Washington, DC.

    Google Scholar 

  • Poizner, H., Klima, E. S., & Bellugi, U. (1987).What the hands reveal about the brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roth, J., & Kosslyn, S. M. (1988). Construction of the third dimension in mental imagery.Cognitive Psychology, 20, 344–361.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sandler, W. (1989).Phonological representation of the sign: Linearity and nonlinearity in American Sign Language. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Foris.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shepard, R., & Metzler, J. (1971). Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects.Science, 171, 701–703.

    Google Scholar 

  • Supalla, S. (1991). Manually coded English: The modality question in signed language development. In P. Siple & S. D. Fischer (Eds.),Theoretical issues in sign language research (Vol. 2, pp. 85–109). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Supalla, T. (1982).Structure and acquisition of verbs of motion and location in American Sign Language. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of California, San Diego.

    Google Scholar 

  • Supalla, T. (1986). The classifier system in American Sign Language. In C. Craig (Ed.),Noun classification and categorization. Amsterdam: J. Benjamins North America.

    Google Scholar 

  • Supalla, T. (in press).Structure and acquisition of verbs of motion and location in American Sign Language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/Bradford Books.

  • van Hoek, K. (1989). Locus splitting in American Sign Language. In R. Carlson, S DeLancey, S. Gildea, D. Payne, & A. Saxens (Eds.),Proceedings of the fourth meeting of the Pacific Linguistics Conference. (pp. 239–255). Eugene: University of Oregon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Hoek, K. (in press). Conceptual locations for referents in ASL. In E. Sweetser & G. Fauconnier (Eds.),Spaces, worlds, and grammars.

  • Wilbur, R. (1987).American sign language: Linguistic and applied dimensions. Boston, MA: College Hill Publications.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

This research was supported in part by National Institutes of Health grant HD-13249 awarded to Ursula Bellugi and Karen Emmorey, as well as NIH grants DC-00146, DC-00201, and HD-26022. I would like to thank and acknowledge Ursula Bellugi for her collaboration during much of the research described in this article.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Emmorey, K. Processing a dynamic visual—Spatial language: Psycholinguistic studies of American Sign Language. J Psycholinguist Res 22, 153–187 (1993). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01067829

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01067829

Keywords

Navigation