Skip to main content
Log in

A technique for measuring processing load during speech production

  • Published:
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

A technique is presented for the measurement of fluctuations in processing demands during spontaneous speech. The technique consists of the analysis of errors on a secondary tracking task. Data are presented from illustrative samples of spontaneous speech; thus, evidence was found to suggest that one level of planning in speech is clauses that contain a single main verb. Evidence was also obtained that there is an increase in processing demands at the gap in subject relative and object relative clauses. It is concluded that the secondary tracking task is a useful technique that could be extended to studies of reading and speech comprehension.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Allport, D.A. (1980). Attention and performance. In G. Claxton (Ed.),Cognitive psychology: New directions. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allport, D.A., Antonis, B., & Reynolds, P. (1972). On the division of attention: A disproof or the single channel hypothesis.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 24, 225–235.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beattie, G. (1983).Talk: An analysis of speech and non-verbal behaviour in conversation. Milton Keynes: Open University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brotherton, P. (1979). Speaking and not speaking: Processes for translating ideas into speech. In A. Siegman & S. Feldstein (Eds.),Of time and speech. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butterworth, B. (1975). Hesitation and semantic planning in speech.Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 4, 75–87.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butterworth, B. (1980). Evidence from pauses in speech. In B. Butterworth (Ed.),Language production, Vol.1: Speech and talk. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butterworth, B., & Beattie, G.W. (1978). Gesture and silence as indicators of planning in speech. In R.N. Campbell & P.T. Smith (Eds.),Recent advances in the psychology of language: Formal and experimental approaches. New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cole, R.A. (1973). Listening for mispronunciations: A measure of what we hear during speech.Perception and Psychophysics, 14, 153–156.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, M.W. (1984).A handbook of cognitive psychology, Hillsdale, New Jersey: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fay, D. (1979). Performing transformations. In R. Cole (Ed.),Perception and production of fluent speech. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J.A., Bever, T.G., & Garrett, M.F. (1974).The psychology of language: An introduction to psycholinguistics and generative grammar. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ford, M. (1980).Sentence planning units: Implications for the speaker's representation of meaningful relations underlying sentences. Unpublished manuscript, MIT.

  • Ford, M. (1983). A method for obtaining measures of local parsing complexity throughout sentences.Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 22, 203–218.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ford, M., & Holmes, V.M. (1978). Planning units and syntax in sentence production.Cognition, 6, 35–53.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frazier, L., Clifton, C., & Randall, J. (1983). Filling gaps: Decision principles and structure in sentence comprehension.Cognition, 13, 187–222.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garrett, M.F. (1976). Syntactic processes in sentence production. In E. Walker & R. Wales (Eds.),New approaches to language mechanisms. Amsterdam: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garrett, M.F. (1980). Levels of processing in sentence production. In B. Butterworth (Ed.),Language production, Vol.1: Speech and talk. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hirst, W., Spelke, E.S., Reaves, C.C., Caharack, G., & Neisser, U. (1980). Dividing attention without alternation and automaticity.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 109, 98–117.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson-Laird, P.N. (1977). Psycholinguistics without linguistics. In N.S. Sutherland (Ed.),Tutorial essays in psychology (Vol.1). New York: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson-Laird, P.N. (1980 June).Propositional representation. Procedural semantics—Mental model. Paper presented at the CNRS Conference, Royaumont.

  • Kowal, S.H., & O'Connell, D.C. (1985). Cognitive rhythms reluctantly revisited.Language and Speech, 28, 93–95.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levelt, W.J.M. (1981, March 11–12).The speakers' linearization problem. Paper presented at the Royal Society/British Academy discussion meeting on the psychological mechanisms of language.

  • Levelt, W.J.M. (1983). Monitoring and self-repair in speech.Cognition, 14, 41–104.

    Google Scholar 

  • McLeod, P. (1977). A dual task response modality effect: Support for multiprocessor models of attention.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 29, 651–667.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norman, D.A., & Bobrow, D.G. (1975). On data-limited and resource-limited processes.Cognitive Psychology, 7, 44–64.

    Google Scholar 

  • Poulton, E.C. (1969). Tracking. In E.A. Bilodeau (Ed.),Principles of skill acquisition. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Power, M.J. (1981).An experimental investigation of spontaneous speech. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Sussex.

  • Power, M.J. (1983). Are there cognitive rhythms in speech?Language and Speech, 26, 253–261.

    Google Scholar 

  • Power, M.J. (1984). Are there cognitive rhythms in speech?—A reply to Beattie (1984).Language and Speech, 27, 197–201.

    Google Scholar 

  • Power, M.J. (1985). Sentence production and working memory.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 37A, 367–385.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shaffer, L.H. (1975). Multiple attention in continuous verbal tasks. In P.M.A. Rabbit & S. Dornic (Eds.),Attention and performance (Vol.V). London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shiffrin, R.M., & Schneider, W. (1984). Automatic and controlled processing revisited.Psychological Review, 91, 269–276.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spelke, E., Hirst, W., & Neisser, U. (1976). Skills of divided attention.Cognition, 4, 215–230.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trumbo, D., Noble, M., & Swink, J. (1967). Secondary task interference in the performance of tracking tasks.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 73, 232–240.

    Google Scholar 

  • Valian, V.V. (1971).Talking, listening, and linguistic structure. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, North Eastern University.

  • Wanner, E., & Maratsos, M. (1978). An ATN approach to comprehension. In M. Halle, J. Bresnan, & G.A. Miller (Eds.),Linguistic theory and psychological reality, Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Power, M.J. A technique for measuring processing load during speech production. J Psycholinguist Res 15, 371–382 (1986). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01067720

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01067720

Keywords

Navigation