Advertisement

Journal of Gambling Studies

, Volume 9, Issue 2, pp 101–120 | Cite as

Fruit machine gambling: The importance of structural characteristics

  • Mark Griffiths
Articles

Abstract

Determinants of the decision to gamble not only include the gambler's biological and psychological constitution but also the structural characteristics of the gambling activity itself. Such characteristics may be responsible for reinforcement, may satisfy gambler's needs, and facilitate excessive gambling. Showing the existence of such relationships has great practical importance. Not only could potentially ‘dangerous’ forms of gambling be identified but effective and selective legislation could be formulated. This paper outlines a history of the importance of structural characteristics in fruit machine gambling and then discusses the role of a number of distinct characteristics including pay out interval, multiplier potential, better involvement, skill, win probability, pay out ratio, suspension of judgement, symbol ratio proportions, the near miss, light, colour, and sound effects and naming. These are all examined in relation to the gambler's behaviour and/or cognitions. It is shown that structural characteristics of fruit machines have the potential to induce excessive gambling regardless of individuals' biological and psychological constitution and that such insights may help in decreasing fruit machine gambling's “addictiveness” potential and help in formulating effective gambling policy.

Keywords

Structural Characteristic Practical Importance Gambling Activity Sound Effect Great Practical Importance 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Abt, V., Smith, J.F. & Christiansen, E.M. (1985).The business of risk: Commercial gambling in mainstream America. Lawrence, Kansas: University of Kansas Press.Google Scholar
  2. Acking, C.A. & Kuller, R. (1972). The perception of an interior as a function of its colour.Ergonomics, 15, 645–654.Google Scholar
  3. American Psychiatric Association (1987).Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, (3rd Edition-revised) Washington D.C.: Author.Google Scholar
  4. Amsel, A. (1958). The role of frustrative non reward in non continuous reward situations.Psychological Bulletin, 55, 102–119.Google Scholar
  5. Birren, F. (1965).Colour psychology and colour therapy. New York Hyde, NY: University Books.Google Scholar
  6. Birren, F. (1978).Colour and human response. New York: Van Nostrand.Google Scholar
  7. Bornstein, M.H. (1978). Colour vision and pattern vision.Advances in Child Development and Behaviour, 12, 117–182.Google Scholar
  8. Boyce, P.R. (1975). The luminous environment. In D. Canter and P. Stringer (Eds.)Environmental interaction: Psychological approaches to our physical surroundings, pp 81–124. New York: International Universities Press.Google Scholar
  9. Caldwell, G. (1974). The gambling Australian. In D.E. Edgar (Ed.)Social change in Australia: Readings in sociology, pp 13–28. Melbourne: Cheshire.Google Scholar
  10. Carr, S.J. & Dabbs, J.M. (1974). The effects of lighting, distance and intimacy of topic on verbal and visual behaviour.Sociornetry, 37, 592–600.Google Scholar
  11. Cornish, D.B. (1978).Gambling: A review of the literature. London: HMSO.Google Scholar
  12. Costa, N. (1988).Automatic pleasures: The history of the coin machine. London: Kevin Francis Publishing.Google Scholar
  13. Eysenck, H.J. (1941). A critical and experimental study of colour preferences.American Journal of Psycholop, 55, 385–394.Google Scholar
  14. Fisher, S. (in press). The pull of the fruit machine: A sociological typology of young players.Sociological Review.Google Scholar
  15. Goethe, J.W. Von (1971).Goethe's colour theory (edited by R. Matthaei, translated by H. Aach). London: Studio Vista.Google Scholar
  16. Graham, J. (1988).Amusement machines: Dependency and delinquency. Home Office Research Study No. 101. London: HMSO.Google Scholar
  17. Greenlees, E.M. (1988).Casino accounting and financial management. Nevada: University of Nevada Press.Google Scholar
  18. Griffiths, M.D. (1988). The gaming industry: A summary of personal communications. Economic Psychology Research Group Report 88/13, University of Exeter.Google Scholar
  19. Griffiths, M.D. (1989). Gambling in children and adolescents.Journal of Gambling Behaviour, 5, 66–83.Google Scholar
  20. Griffiths, M.D. (1990a). The acquisition, development and maintenance of fruit machine gambling in adolescence.Journal of Gambling Studies, 6, 193–204.Google Scholar
  21. Griffiths, M.D. (1990b). The cognitive psychology of gambling.Journal of Gambling Studies, 6, 31–42.Google Scholar
  22. Griffiths, M.D. (1990c). Addiction to fruit machines: A preliminary study among males.Journal of Gambling Studies, 6, 113–126.Google Scholar
  23. Griffiths, M.D. (1990d). Arcade clientele and gaming preferences: A long term study.Perceptual and Motor Skills, 70, 1258.Google Scholar
  24. Griffiths, M.D. (1990e). The role of cognitive bias and skill in fruit machine gambling. In S.E.G. Lea, P. Webley & B. Young (Eds.).Applied economic psychology in the 1990s. Vol. 1. pp. 228–253. Exeter: Washington Singer Press.Google Scholar
  25. Griffiths, M.D. (1990f, September). The observational study of U.K. amusement arcades and its clientele: A longitudinal perspective. Paper presented at the Social Psychology Section Conference, British Psychological Society, Leeds.Google Scholar
  26. Griffiths, M.D. (1991a). Amusement machine playing in childhood and adolescence: A comparative analysis of video games and fruit machines.Journal of Adolescence, 14, 53–73.Google Scholar
  27. Griffiths, M.D. (1991b). The observational study of U.K. amusement arcades.Journal of Community and Applied Social Psychology, 1, 309–320.Google Scholar
  28. Griffiths, M.D. (1991c). The psychobiology of the near miss in fruit machine gambling.Journal of Psychology, 125, 347–357.Google Scholar
  29. Griffiths, M.D. (1991d). The acquisition, development and maintenance of fruit machine gambling in adolescence. Unpublished PhD thesis, University of Exeter.Google Scholar
  30. Griffiths, M.D. (1993). Tolerance in gambling: An objective measure using the psychophysiological analysis of male fruit machine gamblers, Addictive Behaviors, in press.Google Scholar
  31. Griffiths, M.D. & Swift, G. (1992). The use of light and colour in gambling arcades: A small study. Society for the Study of Gambling Newsletter, 21, 16–22.Google Scholar
  32. Hess, H.F. & Diller, J.V. (1969). Motivation for gambling as revealed in the marketing methods of the legitimate gambling industry.Psychological Reports, 25, 19–27.Google Scholar
  33. Holmes, W.F. (1985). Video games: Concepts and latent influences (Part I).F.B.I. Law Enforcement Bulletin, 54(3), 1–9.Google Scholar
  34. Holohan, C.J. (1982).Environmental psychology. New York: Random House.Google Scholar
  35. Ide-Smith, S. & Lea, S.E.G. (1988). Gambling in young adolescents.Journal of Gambling Behaviour, 4, 110–119.Google Scholar
  36. Kahneman, D. & Tversky, A. (1982). The psychology of preferences.Scientific American, January, 136–142.Google Scholar
  37. Langer, E.J. (1975). The illusion of control.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 311–328.Google Scholar
  38. Langer, E.J. & Roth, J. (1975). The illusion of control as a function of the sequence of outcomes in a purely chance task.Journal of Social Psychology, 32, 951–955.Google Scholar
  39. Logan, H.L. & Berger, E. (1961). Measurement of visual information cues.Illuminating Engineering, 56, 393–403.Google Scholar
  40. Mehrabian, A. & Russell, J.A. (1974). The basic emotional impact of environments.Perceptual and Motor Skills, 38, 283–301.Google Scholar
  41. Moran, E. (1987). Gambling among schoolchildren: The impact of the fruit machine. London: National Council on Gambling.Google Scholar
  42. National Housing and Town Planning Council (1988).The use of amusement arcades: A national survey. London: NHTPC.Google Scholar
  43. Odbert, H.S., Karwoski, T.F. & Eckerson, A.B. (1942). Studies in synesthetic thinking: I. Musical and verbal associations of colour and mood.Journal of General Psychology, 26, 153–173.Google Scholar
  44. Reid, R.L. (1986). The psychology of the near miss.Journal of Gambling Behavior, 2, 32–39.Google Scholar
  45. Royal Commission (1951).Report of the Royal Commission on Betting, Lotteries and Gaming 1949–1951. London: HMSO.Google Scholar
  46. Skinner, B.F. (1953).Science and human behavior. New York: MacMillan.Google Scholar
  47. Spectrum Children's Trust (1988).Slot machine playing by children: Results of a survey in Taunton and Minehead. London: SCT.Google Scholar
  48. Stark, G.M., Saunders, D.M. & Wookey, P. (1982). Differential effects of red and blue lighting on gambling behaviour.Current Psychological Research, 2, 95–100.Google Scholar
  49. Stevens, W.R. & Foxell, C.A.P. (1955). Visual acuity.Light and Lighting, 48, 419–424.Google Scholar
  50. Strickland, L.H. & Grote, F.W. (1967). Temporal presentation of winning symbols and slot machine playing.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 74, 10–13.Google Scholar
  51. Weinstein, D. & Deitch, L. (1974).The impact of legalized gambling: The socioeconomic consequences of lotteries and off-track betting. New York: Praeger.Google Scholar
  52. Wexner, L.B. (1954). The degree to which colors (hues) are associated with mood tones.Journal of Applied Psychology, 38, 432–435.Google Scholar
  53. White, S. (1989). Against the odds.Young People Now, April, 26–27.Google Scholar
  54. Wilson, G.D. (1966). Arousal properties of red versus green.Perceptual and Motor Skills, 23, 947–949.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Human Sciences Press, Inc. 1993

Authors and Affiliations

  • Mark Griffiths
    • 1
  1. 1.Psychology DepartmentUniversity of PlymouthDrake Circus, PlymouthEngland

Personalised recommendations