Anderson, K. J. (1981).The current status of the Easterbrook hypothesis. Unpublished manuscript, Northwestern University.
Anderson, K. J. (1988).Impulsivity, caffeine, and task difficulty: A within-subjects test of the Yerkes-Dodson law. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Anderson, K. J. (in press). Arousal and the inverted-U hypothesis: A critique of Neiss's “Reconceptualizing arousal.”Psychological Bulletin.
Anderson, K. J., & Revelle, W. (1982). Impulsivity, caffeine, and proofreading: A test of the Easterbrook hypothesis.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 8 614–624.
Google Scholar
Anderson, K. J., & Revelle, W. (1983). The interactive effects of caffeine, impulsivity and task demands on a visual search task.Personality and Individual Differences, 4 127–134.
Google Scholar
Bacon, S. J. (1974). Arousal and the range of cue utilization.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 102 81–87.
Google Scholar
Battig, W. F., & Montague, W. E. (1969). Category norms for verbal items in 56 categories: A replication and extension of the Connecticut category norms.Journal of Experimental Psychology Monographs, 80(3, pt. 2).
Blake, M. J. F. (1967). Relationship between circadian rhythm of body temperature and introversion-extraversion.Nature, 215 896–897.
Google Scholar
Briggs, G. E. (1974). On the predictor variable for choice reaction time.Memory and Cognition, 2 575–580.
Google Scholar
Broadbent, D. E. (1978). The current state of noise research: Reply to Poulton.Psychological Bulletin, 85 1052–1067.
Google Scholar
Broadhurst, P. L. (1959). The interaction of task difficulty and motivation: The Yerkes-Dodson Law revived.Acta Psychologica, 16 321–338.
Google Scholar
Burrows, D., & Okada, R. (1976). Parallel scanning of physical and category information.Memory and Cognition, 4 31–35.
Google Scholar
Cook, T. D., & Campbell, D. T. (1979).Quasi-experimentation: Design and analysis issues for field settings. Chicago: Rand McNally.
Google Scholar
Cronbach, L. J., & Meehl, P. E. (1955). Construct validity in psychological tests.Psychological Bulletin, 52 281–302.
Google Scholar
Dickman, S. J., & Meyer, D. E. (1988). Impulsivity and speed-accuracy tradeoffs in information processing.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54 274–290.
Google Scholar
Duffy, E. (1962).Activation and behavior. New York: Wiley.
Google Scholar
Easterbrook, J. A. (1959). The effect of emotion on cue utilization and the organization of behavior.Psychological Review, 66 183–201.
Google Scholar
Eysenck, H. J. (1967).The biological basis of personality. Springfield, IL: Charles C Thomas.
Google Scholar
Eysenck, H. J., & Eysenck, M. W. (1985).Personality and individual differences: A natural science approach. New York: Plenum.
Google Scholar
Eysenck, H. J., & Eysenck, S. B. G. (1964).Eysenck Personality Inventory. San Diego, CA: Educational and Industrial Testing Service.
Google Scholar
Eysenck, H. J., & Levey, A. (1972). Conditioning, introversion-extraversion and the strength of the nervous system. In V. D. Nebylitsyn & J. A. Gray (Eds.),Biological bases of individual behavior (pp. 206–220). New York: Academic Press.
Google Scholar
Eysenck, M. W., & Eysenck, M. C. (1979). Memory scanning, introversion-extraversion, and levels of processing.Journal of Research in Personality, 13 305–315.
Google Scholar
Eysenck, S. B. G., & Eysenck, H. J. (1975).Manual of the EPQ (Personality Questionnaire). London: Hodder and Stoughton.
Google Scholar
Folkard, S., Knauth, P., Monk, T. H., & Rutenfranz, J. (1976). The effect of memory load on the circadian variation in performance efficiency under a rapidly rotating shift system.Ergonomics, 19 479–488.
Google Scholar
Frcka, G., & Martin, I. (1987). Is there — or is there not — an influence on impulsiveness on classical eyelid conditioning?Personality and Individual Differences, 8 241–252.
Google Scholar
Gilbert, R. M. (1976). Caffeine as a drug of abuse. In R. G. Gibbons, Y. Israel, H. Kalant, R. E. Popham, W. Schmit, & R. G. Smart (Eds.)Research advances in alcohol and drug problems, (Vol. 3, pp. 47–176). New York: Wiley.
Google Scholar
Hebb, D. O. (1955). Drives and the C. N. S. (conceptual nervous system).Psychological Review, 62 243–254.
Google Scholar
Hockey, G. R. J. (1970a). Changes in attention allocation in a multicomponent task under loss of sleep.British Journal of Psychology, 61 473–480.
Google Scholar
Hockey, G. R. J. (1970b). Effect of loud noise on attentional selectivity.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 22 28–36.
Google Scholar
Hockey, G. R. J. (1970c). Signal probability and spatial location as possible bases of increased selectivity in noise.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 22 37–42.
Google Scholar
Humphreys, M. S., & Revelle, W. (1984). Personality, motivation, and performance: A theory of the relationship between individual differences and information processing.Psychological Review, 91 153–184.
Google Scholar
Lacey, J. L. (1967). Somatic patterning and stress: Some revisions of activation theory. In M. H. Appley & R. Trumbell (Eds.),Psychological stress (pp. 14–37). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Google Scholar
Loo, R. (1980). Role of primary personality factors in the perception of traffic signs and driver violations and accidents.Accident Analysis and Prevention, 11 125–127.
Google Scholar
McClelland, J. L. (1979). On the time relations of mental processes: An examination of systems of processes in cascade.Psychological Review, 86 287–330.
Google Scholar
O'Gorman, J. G., & Lloyd, J. E. M. (1987). Extraversion, impulsiveness, and EEG alpha activity.Personality and Individual Differences, 8 169–174.
Google Scholar
Poulton, E. C. (1978). A new look at the effects of noise: A rejoinder.Psychological Bulletin, 85 1068–1079.
Google Scholar
Rall, T. W. (1980). Central nervous system stimulants: The xanthines. In L. S. Goodman & A. Gilman (Eds.),Pharmacological basis of therapeutics (6th ed. pp. 592–607). New York: Macmillan.
Google Scholar
Ratcliff, R. (1978). A theory of memory retrieval.Psychological Review, 85 59–108.
Google Scholar
Revelle, W., Amaral, P., & Turriff, S. (1976). Introversion/extroversion, time stress, and caffeine: Effect on verbal performance.Science, 192 149–150.
Google Scholar
Revelle, W., & Anderson, K. J. (in press). Models for the testing of theory. In A. Gale & M. W. Eysenck (Eds.),Handbook of individual differences: Biological perspectives. New York: Wiley.
Revelle, W., Anderson, K. J., & Humphreys, M. S. (1987). Empirical tests and theoretical extensions of arousal-based theories of personality. In J. Strelau & H. J. Eysenck (Eds.),Personality dimensions and arousal (pp. 17–36). New York: Plenum.
Google Scholar
Revelle, W., Humphreys, M. S., Simon, L., & Gilliland, K. (1980). The interactive effect of personality, time of day and caffeine: A test of the arousal model.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 109 1–31.
Google Scholar
Rocklin, T., & Revelle, W. (1981). The measurement of extraversion: A comparison of the Eysenck Personality Inventory and the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire.British Journal of Social Psychology, 20 279–284.
Google Scholar
Shiffrin, R. M., & Schneider, W. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending, and a general theory.Psychological Review, 84 127–190.
Google Scholar
Stelmack, R. M. (1981). The psychophysiology of extraversion and neuroticism. In H. J. Eysenck (Ed.),A model for personality (pp. 38–64). Berlin: Springer.
Google Scholar
Sternberg, S. (1966). High-speed scanning in human memory.Science, 153 652–654.
Google Scholar
Sternberg, S. (1975). Memory scanning: New findings and current controversies.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 27 1–32.
Google Scholar
Walley, R. E., & Weiden, T. D. (1973). Lateral inhibition and cognitive masking: A neuropsychological theory of attention.Psychological Review, 80 284–302.
Google Scholar
Wickelgren, W. A. (1977).Learning and memory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Google Scholar
Yerkes, R. M., & Dodson, J. D., (1908). The relation of strength of stimuli to rapidity of habit-formation.Journal of Comparative Neurology and Psychology, 18 459–482.
Google Scholar