Advertisement

Motivation and Emotion

, Volume 10, Issue 1, pp 11–24 | Cite as

False autonomic feedback: Effects of attention to feedback on ratings of erotic stimuli

  • Brian Parkinson
  • A. S. R. Manstead
Articles

Abstract

This study examines the roles played by sound track type, attention to sound track, and meaning of sound track in mediating the effects of false autonomic feedback on attractiveness ratings of erotic stimuli. Male subjects were instructed either to ignore or to pay attention to a pulsed or continuous sound track that was described either as heart-rate feedback or as a neutral auditory stimulus while slides of nude females were shown. Slides associated with a change in either the pulsed sound track or the continuous-tone sound track (increase slides) were subsequently rated as significantly more attractive than those associated with steady sound (stable slides). This effect was contingent on the meaning given to the auditory stimuli, with subjects in the “heart-rate” condition showing a stronger tendency to rate increase slides more positively than stable slides, by comparison with subjects in the “neutral sounds” condition. Within the heart-rate condition, subjects told to pay attention to the feedback showed greater rating differences between these two types of slide than those told to ignore it. This pattern of findings contrasts with those of an earlier experiment (Parkinson & Manstead, 1981), where differential unpleasantness ratings of slides of skin diseases depended on the attention paid to the sound track but not on its meaning. It is concluded that the effects of false autonomic feedback are contingent upon the kind of emotional stimuli that are presented.

Keywords

Early Experiment Skin Disease Male Subject Auditory Stimulus Strong Tendency 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Barefoot, J. C., & Straub, R. B. (1971). Opportunity for information-search and the effect of false heart-rate feedback.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 17 154–157.Google Scholar
  2. Hirschman, R., & Clark, M. (1983). Bogus physiological feedback. In J. T. Cacioppo & R. E. Petty (Eds.),Social psychophysiology: A sourcebook. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  3. Hirschman, R., Clark, M., & Hawk, G. (1977). Relative effects of bogus physiological feedback and control stimuli on autonomic and self-report indicants of emotional attribution.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 3 270–275.Google Scholar
  4. Kerber, K. W., & Coles, M. G. H. (1978). The role of perceived physiological activity in affecttive judgments.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 14 419–433.Google Scholar
  5. Liebhart, E. M. (1979). Information search and attribution: Cognitive processes mediating the effect of false autonomic feedback.European Journal of Social Psychology, 9 19–36.Google Scholar
  6. Nisbett, R. E., & Valins, S. (1972). Perceiving the causes of one's own behavior. In E. E. Jones, D. E. Kanouse, H. H. Kelley, R. E. Nisbett, S. Valins, & B. Weiner (Eds.),Attribution: Perceiving the causes of behavior. Morristown, New Jersey: General Learning Press.Google Scholar
  7. Oliver, E. (1978). Attention and causal attributions to false heart-rate feedback.Representative Research in Social Psychology, 9 2–13.Google Scholar
  8. Parkinson, B. (1985). Emotional effects of false autonomic feedback.Psychological Bulletin, 98 471–494.Google Scholar
  9. Parkinson, B., & Manstead, A. S. R. (1981). An examination of the roles played by meaning of feedback and attention to feedback in the “Valins effect.”Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40 239–245.Google Scholar
  10. Pennebaker, J. W. (1982).The psychology of physical symptoms. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
  11. Stern, R. M., Botto, R. W., & Herrick, C. D. (1972). Behavioral and physiological effects of false heart-rate feedback: A replication and extension.Psychophysiology, 9 21–29.Google Scholar
  12. Truax, S. R. (1983). Active search, mediation, and the manipulation of cue dimensions: Emotion attribution in the false feedback paradigm.Motivation and Emotion, 7 41–59.Google Scholar
  13. Valins, S. (1966). Cognitive effects of false heart-rate feedback.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 4 400–408.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Publishing Corporation 1986

Authors and Affiliations

  • Brian Parkinson
    • 1
  • A. S. R. Manstead
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of ManchesterManchesterEngland

Personalised recommendations