Motivation and Emotion

, Volume 14, Issue 2, pp 107–130 | Cite as

The origins of empathic concern

  • Carolyn Zahn-Waxler
  • Marian Radke-Yarrow

Abstract

Recent developments in research cast doubt on early conceptions of young children as primarily egocentric and uncaring of others' needs. Studies reviewed indicate a broad range of social competencies children bring to their interpersonal relationships. As early as 2 years of age, they show (a) the cognitive capacity to interpret, in simple ways, the physical and psychological states of others, (b) the emotional capacity to experience, affectively, the state of others, and (c) the behavioral repertoire that permits the possibility of attempts to alleviate discomfort in others. Both temperament and environment may contribute to individual differences in concern for others. Early socialization experiences that lead to adaptive and maladaptive patterns of responsiveness to others' needs are described. Examples of environmental risk conditions include parental depression, marital discord, parental maltreatment. Implications of this work for broadening existing conceptualizations of empathy and related prosocial orientations are addressed.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association (1987).Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed., revised)., Washington, DC: Author.Google Scholar
  2. Amsterdam, B. (1972). Mirror self-image reactions before age two.Developmental Psychology, 8 297–305.Google Scholar
  3. Barnett, M. A., McMinimy, V., Flouer, G., & Masbad, I. (1987). Adolescent's evaluations of peers' motives for helping.Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 16 579–586.Google Scholar
  4. Batson, C. D., Batson, J. G., Griffitt, C. A., Barrientos, S., Brandt, J. R., Spreugelmeyer, P. & Bayly, M. J. (1989). Negative-state relief and the empathy-altruism hypothesis.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56 922–933.Google Scholar
  5. Batson, C. D., Dyck, J. L., Brandt, J. R., Batson, J. G., Powell, A. L., McMaster, M. R., & Griffith, C. A. (1988). Five studies testing two new egoistic alternatives to the empathy-altruism hypothesis.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55 52–77.Google Scholar
  6. Batson, C. D., & Shaw, L. L. (in press). Evidence for altruism: Toward a pluralism of prosocial motives.Psychology Inquiry.Google Scholar
  7. Beardslee, W., & Podorefsky, D. (1988). Resilient adolescents whose parents have serious affective and other psychiatric disorders: Importance of self-understanding and relationships.American Journal of Psychiatry, 145 63–69.Google Scholar
  8. Beardslee, W. R., Schultz, L. H., & Selman, R. L., (1987). Level of social-cognitive development, adoptive functioning and DSM-III diagnoses in adolescent offspring of parents with affective disorders: Implications for the development of the capacity for mutuality.Developmental Psychology, 23 807–815.Google Scholar
  9. Bertenthal, B. I., & Fischer, K. W. (1978). the development of self-recognition in the infant.Developmental Psychology, 14 44–50.Google Scholar
  10. Bloom, L. P., & Hood, L. (1975). Structure and variation in child language.Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 40 (3–4, serial no. 161).Google Scholar
  11. Bretherton, I., Fritz, J., Zahn-Waxler, C., & Ridgeway, D. (1986). The acquisition and development of emotion language: A functionalist perspective.Child Development, 57 529–548.Google Scholar
  12. Brothers, L. (1989). A biological perspective on empathy.The American Journal of Psychiatry, 146 10–19.Google Scholar
  13. Bruner, J. S. (1972). The nature and uses of immaturity.American Psychologist, 27 1–22.Google Scholar
  14. Butterworth, G. (1980). A discussion of some issues raised by Piaget's concept of childhood egocentrism. In M. V. Cox (Ed.),Are young children egocentric? (pp. 17–40). York, England: Batsford Academic and Educational.Google Scholar
  15. Campos, J. J., Barrett, K. C., Lamb, M. E., Goldsmith, H. H., & Stenberg, C. (1983). Socio-emotional development. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.),Handbook of child psychology (4th ed., pp. 783–915) New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  16. Caplan, M. Z., & Hay, D. F. (1989). Preschoolers' responses to peers' distress and beliefs about bystander intervention.Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30 231–242.Google Scholar
  17. Carlson, M., & Miller, N. (1987). Examination of the relation between negative mood and helping.Psychological Bulletin, 102 91–108.Google Scholar
  18. Cialdini, R. B., Schaller, M., Houlihan, D., Arps, K., Fultz, J., & Beaman, A. L. (1987). Empathy-based helping: Is it selflessly motivated?Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52 749–758.Google Scholar
  19. Cicchetti, D., & Schneider-Rosen, K. (1986). An organizational approach to childhood depression. In M. Rutter, C. Izard, & P. Read (Eds.),Depression in young people: Clinical and developmental perspectives. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  20. Cohn, J. F., & Tronick, E. Z. (1983). Three-month-old infants' reaction to simulated maternal depression.Child Development, 54 185–193.Google Scholar
  21. Cohn, J. K., Campbell, S. B., Matias, R., & Hopkins, J. (1990). Face-to-face interactions of postpartum depressed and non-depressed mother-infant pairs at 2 months.Developmental Psychology, 26 15–23.Google Scholar
  22. Cooley, C. H. (1926). The roots of social knowledge.American Journal of Sociology, 32 59–79.Google Scholar
  23. Cummings, E. M., Hollenbeck, B., Iannotti, R., Radke-Yarrow, M., & Zahn-Waxler, C. (1986). Early organization of altruism and aggression: Developmental patterns and individual difference. In C. Zahn-Waxler, E. M. Cummings, & R. Iannotti (Eds.),Altruism and aggression: Biological and social origins (pp. 165–188). New York, London, & Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  24. Cummings, E. M., Zahn-Waxler, C., & Radke-Yarrow, M. (1981). Young children's responses to expressions of anger and affection by others in the family.Child Development, 52 1274–1282.Google Scholar
  25. Cummings, E. M., Zahn, Waxler, C., & Radke-Yarrow, M. (1984). Developmental changes in children's reactions to anger in the home.Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 25 63–74.Google Scholar
  26. Darwin, C. (1872).The expression of the emotions in man and animal. London: John Murray.Google Scholar
  27. Dawkins, R. (1976).The selfish gene. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  28. Eckerman, C. O., Whatley, J. L., & Kutz, S. L. (1975). Growth of social play with peers during the second year of life.Developmental Psychology, 11 42–49.Google Scholar
  29. Eisenberg, N., & Lennon, R. (1980). Altruism and the assessment of empathy in the preschool years.Child Development, 51 552–557.Google Scholar
  30. Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Miller, P. A., Fultz, J., Shell, R., Mathy, R. M., & Reno, R. R. (1989a). Relation of sympathy and personal distress to prosocial behavior: A multi-method study.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57 55–66.Google Scholar
  31. Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Shaller, M., & Miller, P. A. (1989b). Sympathy and personal distress: Development, gender differences, and interrelations of indexes. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.),Empathy and related emotion responses (New Directions in Child Development, No. 44, pp. 107–126). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
  32. Eisenberg, N., & Strayer, J. (1987).Empathy and its development. Cambridge, England, & New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  33. Emde, R., Johnson, W., & Easterbrooks, M. A. (1987). The do's and don'ts of early moral development: Psychoanalytic tradition and current research. In J. Kagan, & S. Lamb (Eds.),The emergence of morality in young children (pp. 245–276). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  34. Emery, R. E. (1982). Interparent conflict and the children of discord and divorce.Psychological Bulletin, 92, 310–330.Google Scholar
  35. Fabes, R. A., Eisenberg, N., & Miller, P. (1990). Maternal correlates of children's vicarious emotional responsiveness.Developmental Psychology, 26, 639–648.Google Scholar
  36. Field, T., Woodson, R., Greenberg, R., & Cohen, D. (1982). Discrimination and imitation of facial expressions by neonates.Science, 218, 179–181.Google Scholar
  37. Freud, S. (1958).Civilization and its discontents. New York: Doubleday Anchor Books.Google Scholar
  38. Gladstein, G. A. (1984). The historical roots of contemporary empathy research.Journal of the History off the Behavioral Sciences, 20, 38–59.Google Scholar
  39. Haviland, J. M., & Lelivica, M. (1987). The induced affect response: 10-week-old infants' responses to three emotion expressions.Developmental Psychology, 23, 97–104.Google Scholar
  40. Haynes-Seman, C. (1987). Developmental origins of moral masochism: A failure-to-thrive toddler's interactions with mother.Child Abuse and Neglect, 11, 319–330.Google Scholar
  41. Hoffman, M. L. (1975). Developmental synthesis of affect and cognition and its interplay for altruistic motivation.Developmental Psychology, 11, 607, 622.Google Scholar
  42. Hoffman, M. (1982). Development of prosocial motivation: Empathy and guilt. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.),The development of prosocial behavior (pp. 281–313). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  43. Howes, C., & Eldridge, R. (1985). Responses off abused, neglected and nonmaltreated children to the behaviors of their peers.Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 6, 261–270.Google Scholar
  44. Howes, C., & Parver, J. (1987). Toddlers' responses to the distress of their peers.Journal of applied Developmental Psychology, 8, 441–452.Google Scholar
  45. Izard, C. E. (1977).Human emotion. New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
  46. Kagan, J., & Lamb, S. (1987).The emergence of morality in young children. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  47. Klimes-Dougan, B., & Kistner, J. (1990). Physically abused preschoolers' responses to peers' distress.Developmental Psychology 26, 599–602.Google Scholar
  48. Koestner, R., Franz, C., & Weinberger, J. (1990). The family origins of empathic concern: A 26-year longitudinal study.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 709–717.Google Scholar
  49. Lewis, M., Sullivan, M., Stanger, C., & Weiss, M. (1989). Self development and self-conscious emotions.Child Development, 60, 146–156.Google Scholar
  50. MacLean, P. D. (1985). Brain evolution relating to family, play, and the separation call.Archives of General Psychiatry, 42, 405–417.Google Scholar
  51. Mahler, M. S., Pine, F., & Bergman, A. (1975).The psychological birth of the human infant: Symbiosis and individuation. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  52. Main, M., & George, C. (1985). Response of abused and disadvantaged toddlers to distress in playmates: A study in the day care setting.Developmental Psychology, 21, 407–412.Google Scholar
  53. Main, M., & Goldwyn, R. (1984). Predicting rejection of her infant from mother's representation of her own experience: Implications for the abused-abusing intergenerational cycle.Child Abuse and Neglect, 8, 203–217.Google Scholar
  54. Matas, L., Arend, R. A., & Sroufe, L. A. (1978). Continuity of adaptation in the second year: The relationship between quality of attachment and later competence.Child Development, 49, 547–556.Google Scholar
  55. Mathews, K. A., Batson, C. D., Horn, J., & Rosenman, R. H. (1981). Principles in his nature which interest him in the fortune of others: The heritability of empathic concern for others.Journal of Personality, 49, 237–247.Google Scholar
  56. McCall, R. B. (1979). Qualitative transitions in behavioral development in the first three years. In M. H. Bornstein & W. Kessen (Eds.),Psychological development in infancy. New York: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  57. Mead, C. H. (1934).Mind, self and society. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  58. Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates.Science, 198, 75–78.Google Scholar
  59. Miller, P. A., & Eisenberg, N. (1988). The relation of empathy to aggressive and externalizing/antisocial behavior.Psychological Bulletin, 103, 324–344.Google Scholar
  60. Nicolich, L. (1977). Beyond sensorimotor intelligence: Assessment of symbolic maturity through analyses of pretend play.Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 23, 89–99.Google Scholar
  61. Piaget, J. (1962).Play, dreams and imitation in childhood. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
  62. Piaget, J. (1965).The moral judgment of the child. New York: Harcourt, Brace. (Original work published in 1932).Google Scholar
  63. Radke-Yarrow, M., & Zahn-Waxler, C. (1976). Dimensions and correlates of prosocial behavior in young children.Child Development, 47, 118–125.Google Scholar
  64. Radke-Yarrow, M., & Zahn-Waxler, C. (1984). Roots, motives, and patterning in children's prosocial behavior. In E. Staub, K. D. Bartal, J. Karylowski, & J. Raykowski (Eds.),The development and maintenance of prosocial behavior: International perspectives on positive morality. New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
  65. Radke-Yarrow, M., Zahn-Waxler, C., & Chapman, M. (1983). Children's prosocial dispositions and behavior. In P. H. Mussen, & E. M. Hetherington (Eds.),Handbook of child psychology: Socialization, personality, and social development (Vol. 4, pp. 469–545), New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  66. Rheingold, H. L., & Emery, G. N. (1986). The nurturant acts of very young children. In D. Olweus, J. Block, & M. Radke-Yarrow (Eds.),The development of anti- and prosocial behavior. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  67. Rheingold, H., Hay, D., & West, M. (1976). Sharing in the second year of life.Child Development, 47, 1148–1158.Google Scholar
  68. Ross, H. S., & Goldman, B. D. (1977). Establishing new social relations in infancy. In T. Alloway, L. Kramer, & P. Plinerf (Eds.),Advances in the study of communication and affect: Vol. 3. Attachment behavior. New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
  69. Rushton, J. P., Fulker, D. W., Neale, M. C., Nias, D. L. B., & Eysenck, H. J. (1986). Altruism and aggression: The heritability of individual differences.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 1192–1198.Google Scholar
  70. Rushton, J. P., Russell, R. J. H., & Wells, P. A. (1984). Genetic similarity theory: Beyond kin selection.Behavior Genetics, 14, 179–193.Google Scholar
  71. Sagi, A., & Hoffman, M. L. (1976). Empathic distress in the newborn.Developmental Psychology, 12, 175–176.Google Scholar
  72. Shott, S. (1979). Emotion and social life: A symbolic interactionist analysis.American Journal of Sociology, 84, 1317–1334.Google Scholar
  73. Simner, M. L. (1971). Newborn's responses to the cry of another infant.Developmental Psychology, 5, 136–150.Google Scholar
  74. Stern, D. (1985).The interpersonal world of the infant. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  75. Stipek, D. J., Gralinski, J. H., & Kopp, C. B. (1990). Self-concept development in the toddler years.Developmental Psychology, 26, 972–977.Google Scholar
  76. Strayer, J., & Roberts, W. (1989). Children's empathy an role taking: Child and parental factors and relations to prosocial behavior.Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 10, 227–239.Google Scholar
  77. Sullivan, H. S. (1947).Conceptions of modern psychiatry. Washington, DC: White Foundation.Google Scholar
  78. Termine, N. T., & Izard, C. E. (1988). Infants' responses to their mothers' expressions of joy and sadness.Developmental Psychology, 24, 223–229.Google Scholar
  79. Teti, D. M., & Ablard, K. A. (1991). Security of attachment and infant-sibling relationships: A laboratory study.Child Development.Google Scholar
  80. Trevarthen, C. (1989). Origins and directions for the concept of infant intersubjectivity.Society for Research in Child Development, Newsletter.Google Scholar
  81. Watson, M., & Fischer, K. (1977). A developmental sequence of agent use in late infancy.Child Development, 48, 828–836.Google Scholar
  82. Weisenfeld, A. R., Whitman, P. B., & Malatesta, C. Z. (1984). Individual differences among adult women in sensitivity to infants: Evidence in support of an empathy construct.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 118–124.Google Scholar
  83. Wilson, E. O. (1975).Sociobiology: The new synthesis. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  84. Yarrow, M., Scott, P., & Waxler, C. (1973). Learning concern for other.Developmental Psychology, 8, 240–260.Google Scholar
  85. Zahn-Waxler, C. Cole, P., & Barrett, K. C. (1991). Guilt and empathy: Sex differences and implications for the development of depression. In K. Dodge & J. Garber (Eds.),Emotion regulation and dysregulation. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  86. Zahn-Waxler, C., & Kochanska, G. (1990). The origins of guilt. In R. Thompson (Ed.),Socioemotional development (pp. 183–258). Lincoln: Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
  87. Zahn-Waxler, C., Kochanska, G. Krupnick, J., & McKnew, D. (1990). Patterns of guilt in children of depressed and well mothers.Developmental Psychology, 26, 51–59.Google Scholar
  88. Zahn-Waxler, C., & Radke-Yarrow, M. (1982). The development of altruism: Alternative research strategies. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.),The development of prosocial behavior (pp. 109–137). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  89. Zahn-Waxler, C. Radke-Yarrow, M., & King, R. A. (1979). Child-rearing and children's prosocial initiations toward victims of distress.Child Development, 50, 319–330.Google Scholar
  90. Zahn-Waxler, C., Radke-Yarrow, M., Chapman, M., & Wagner, E. (1990).The development of concern for others. Unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar
  91. Zahn-Waxler, C., Robinson, J., & Emde, R. (1991). Thedevelopment and heritability of empathy. Poster presentation, Society for Research in Child Development, Seattle, WA, April.Google Scholar
  92. Zahn-Waxler, C., & Smith, K. D. (in press). The development of prosocial behavior. In V. B. Van Hasselt and M. Herson (Eds.),Handbook of Social Development: A lifespan perspective, New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Publishing Corporation 1990

Authors and Affiliations

  • Carolyn Zahn-Waxler
    • 1
  • Marian Radke-Yarrow
    • 1
  1. 1.Laboratory of Developmental Psychology,Building 15-KNational Institute of Mental HealthBethesda

Personalised recommendations