Advertisement

Journal of Chemical Ecology

, Volume 17, Issue 7, pp 1293–1307 | Cite as

Chemical orientation of lobsters, homarus americanus, in turbulent odor plumes

  • Paul A. Moore
  • Nat Scholz
  • Jelle Atema
Article

Abstract

The lobster,Homarus americanus, relies upon its lateral antennules to make initial directional choices in a turbulent odor plume. To determine whether chemical signals provide cues for source direction and distance during orientation, we studied the search patterns of the lobster orienting within a turbulent odor plume. In an odor plume, animals walked significantly more slowly and most often up the middle of the tank; control animals (no odor present) walked rapidly in straight lines, frequently along a wall. Search patterns were not stereotyped either for the population of experimental animals or for individuals. Three different phases of orientation were evident: an initial stage during which the animals increased their walking speeds and decreased their heading angles; an intermediate stage where both the walking speed and headings were constant; and the final stage close to the source, where heading angles increased while walking speed decreased. During this last stage the animals appear to be switching from a distance orientation (mediated by the antennules) to a local food search (mediated by the walking legs) as evidenced by a great increase in leg-raking behavior.

Key Words

Chemotaxis chemoreception orientation lobster Homarus americanus odor plume 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Akers, R.P. 1989. Counterturns initiated by decrease in rate of increase of concentration: possible mechanism of chemotaxis by walking female Ips paraconfusus bark beetle.J. Chem. Ecol. 15:183–208.Google Scholar
  2. Atema, J. 1985. Chemoreception in the sea: adaptation of chemoreceptors and behavior to aquatic stimulus conditions.Soc. Exp. Biol. Symp. 39:387–423.Google Scholar
  3. Atema, J. 1988. Distribution of chemical stimuli, pp. 29–56, in J. Atema, A.N. Popper, R.R. Fay, and W.N. Tavolga (eds.). Sensory Biology of Aquatic Animals, Springer-Verlag, New York.Google Scholar
  4. Atema, J., andCobb, J.S. 1980. Social behavior, pp. 409–450, in J.S. Cobb, and B.F. Phillips (eds.). The Biology and Management of Lobster, Vol. I, Academic Press, New York.Google Scholar
  5. Baker, T.C., Willis, M.A., andPhelan, P.L. 1984. Optomotor anemotaxis polarizes self-steered zigzagging in flying moths.Physiol. Entomol. 9:365–376.Google Scholar
  6. Bell, W.J. 1984. Chemo-orientation in walking insects, pp. 93–106, in W.J. Bell, and R.T. Cardé (eds.). Chemical Ecology of Insects, Sinauer Associates, Inc., Sunderland, Massachusetts.Google Scholar
  7. Bell, W.J. andCardé, R.T. 1984. Chemical Ecology of Insects. Sinauer Associates, Inc., Sunderland, Massachusetts.Google Scholar
  8. Bell, W.J., andTobin, T.R. 1982. Chemo-orientation.Biol. Rev. 57:219–260.Google Scholar
  9. Borroni, P., Handrich, L., andAtema, J. 1986. The role of narrowly tuned taste cell populations in lobster (Homarus americanus) feeding behavior.Behav. Neurol. 100:206–212.Google Scholar
  10. Bursell, E. 1984. Observations on the orientation of tsetse flies (Glossinia pallipdipes) to windborne odours.Physiol. Entomol. 9:133–137.Google Scholar
  11. David, C.T. 1986. Mechanisms of directional flight in wind, pp. 49–58, in T.L. Payne, M.C. Birch, and C.E.J. Kennedy (eds.). Mechanisms in Insect Olfaction, Clarendon Press, Oxford. 364 pp.Google Scholar
  12. Derby, C.D., andAtema, J. 1982. The function of chemo- and mechanoreceptors in lobster (Homarus americanus) feeding behavior.J. Exp. Biol. 98:317–327.Google Scholar
  13. Devine, D.V., andAtema, J. 1982. Function of chemoreceptor organs in spatial orientation of the lobster, Homarus americanus: Differences and overlap.Biol. Bull. 163:144–153.Google Scholar
  14. Fraenkel, G.S., andGunn, D.L. 1961. The Orientation of Animals,; 2nd ed. Dover Publishers, New York.Google Scholar
  15. Havukkala, I.J., andKennedy, J.S. 1984. A programme of self-steered turns as a humidity response in Tenebrio, and the problem of categorizing spatial manoeuvres.Physiol. Entomol. 9:157–164.Google Scholar
  16. Hodgson, E.S., andMatthewson, R.F. 1971. Chemosensory orientation in sharks.Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 188:175–182.Google Scholar
  17. Kennedy, J.S. 1978. The concepts of olfactory “arrestment” and “attraction.” Physiol.Entomol. 3:91–98.Google Scholar
  18. Kennedy, J.S. 1982. Mechanism of moth attraction: a modified view based on wind tunnel experiments with flying male Adoxophyes. les Colloq.INRA 7:189–192.Google Scholar
  19. Kennedy, J.S. 1986. Some current issues in orientation to odour sources, pp. 11–25, in T.L. Payne, M.C. Birch, and C.E.J. Kennedy (eds.). Mechanisms in Insect Olfaction, Clarendon Press, Oxford. 364 pp.Google Scholar
  20. Kleerekoper, H. 1967. Some aspects of olfaction in fish, with special reference to orientation.Am. Zool. 7:385–295.Google Scholar
  21. Kleerekoper, H., Timms, A.M., Westlake, G.F., Davy, F.B., Malar, T., Anderson, V.M. 1969. Inertial guidance system in the orientation of the goldfish (Carassius auratus).Nature 223:501–502.Google Scholar
  22. Kramer, E. 1986. Turbulent diffusion and pheromone-triggered anemotaxis, pp. 59–68, in T.L. Payne, M.C. Birch, and C.E.J. Kennedy (eds.). Mechanisms in Insect Olfaction, Clarendon Press, Oxford. 364 pp.Google Scholar
  23. Kúhn, A. 1919. Die Orientierung der Tiere im Raum. Gustav Fischer Verlag, Jena.Google Scholar
  24. Loeb, J. 1891. Über Geotropismus bei Tieren.Pflügers Arch. 49:175–189.Google Scholar
  25. Loeb, J. 1913. Die Tropismen. Hdb. d. vergl. Physiol. Bd. IV. pp. 451–511. Hrsg.: H. Winterstein. Gustav Fischer, Jena.Google Scholar
  26. Loeb, J. 1918. Forced Movements, Tropisms and Animal Conduct. Philadelphia. 209 pp.Google Scholar
  27. Moore, P.A., andAtema, J. 1988. A model of a temporal filter in chemoreception to extract directional information from a turbulent odor plume.Biol. Bull. 174:355–363.Google Scholar
  28. Moore, P.A., Gerhardt, G.A., andAtema, J. 1989. High resolution spatio-temporal analysis of aquatic chemical signals using microelectrochemical electrodes.Chem. Senses 14:829–840.Google Scholar
  29. Murlis, J. 1986. The structure of odour plumes, pp. 27–38, in T.L. Payne, M.C. Birch, and C.E.J. Kennedy (eds.). Mechanisms in Insect Olfaction, Clarendon Press, Oxford. 364 pp.Google Scholar
  30. Murlis, J., andJones, C.D. 1981. Fine-scale structure of odour plumes in relation to insect orientation to distant pheromone and other attractant sources.Physiol. Entomol. 6:71–86.Google Scholar
  31. Payne, T.L., Birch, M.C., andKennedy, C.E.J. (eds.). 1986. Mechanisms in Insect Olfaction. Clarendon Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
  32. Preiss, R., andKramer, E. 1986. Pheromone-induced anemotaxis in simulated free flight, pp. 69–80, in T.L. Payne, M.C. Birch, and C.E.J. Kennedy (eds.). Mechanisms in Insect Olfaction, Clarendon Press, Oxford. 364 pp.Google Scholar
  33. Schöne, H. 1984. Spatial Orientation. Princeton University Press, Princeton, New Jersey.Google Scholar
  34. Reeder, P.B., andAche, B.W. 1980. Chemotaxis in the Florida spiny lobster, Panulirus argus.Anim. Behav. 28:831–839.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Publishing Corporation 1991

Authors and Affiliations

  • Paul A. Moore
    • 1
  • Nat Scholz
    • 1
  • Jelle Atema
    • 1
  1. 1.Marine Biological LaboratoryBoston University Marine ProgramWoods Hole

Personalised recommendations