Advertisement

Mood and memories of parental rearing styles: A comparison of mood effects on questionnaire-cued and free recall of autobiographical memories

  • Coby Gerlsma
  • Ineke Mosterman
  • Sytske Buwalda
  • Paul M. G. Emmelkamp
Article

Abstract

The relationship between memories of childhood experiences (e.g., adverse parenting) and adult depression often found raises questions of interpretation. On the one hand, both laboratory studies and clinicians' experiences suggest that subjects in a depressed mood frequently show a negative bias in perceptions and memories. Negative childhood memories in depressed persons might, therefore, be interpreted as epiphenomena of depressed mood instead of etiological factors. On the other hand, memories of childhood experiences seem remarkably stable across changes in depressed mood, especially when memories are elicited by means of standardized questionnaires. In the mood and memory literature several explanations for this stability are offered. For one thing, highly structured cues to elicit memories (such as in questionnaires) are hypothesized to be less susceptible to mood bias than unstructured memory cues (such as in free recall procedures). On the other hand, resource allocation theorists suggest that childhood memories, being well established and rehearsed, are relatively impervious to mood bias no matter how they are elicited. In this study we examined whether different methods of eliciting childhood memories (i.e., free recall and questionnaire-cued) are differentially susceptible to mood bias. To this aim, we used a mood induction procedure to induce depressed, neutral, and elated mood and assessed childhood memories both before and after the mood induction using both questionnaires and free recall to elicit memories. Results suggested that memories elicited by means of free recall as well as by means of questionnaire-cued recall were susceptible to depressed and elated mood bias. The implications for research addressing the link between childhood experiences and depression are discussed.

Key words

mood effects childhood memories parental rearing styles recall methods 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Ackermann, R., & DeRubeis, R. J. (1991). Is depressive realism real?Clinical Psychology Review, 11, 565–584.Google Scholar
  2. Albersnagel, F. A. (1987). Velten and musical induction procedures: A comparison with accessibility of thought associations. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 26, 79–96.Google Scholar
  3. Alloy, L. B., & Abramson, L. Y. (1979). Judgment of contingency in depressed and nondepressed students: Sadder but wiser?Journal of Experimental Psychology, 108, 441–485.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Beck, A. T. (1967).Depression: Clinical, experimental and theoretical aspects. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
  5. Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J., & Erbaugh, J. (1961). An inventory for measuring depression.Archives of General Psychiatry, 4, 561–571.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F., & Emery, B. (1979).Cognitive therapy of depression. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  7. Blaney, P. H. (1986). Affect and memory: A review.Psychological Bulletin, 99, 229–246.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Blatt, S. J., & Homann, E. (1992). Parent-child interaction in the etiology of depression.Clinical Psychology Review, 12, 47–91.Google Scholar
  9. Blatt, S. J., & Maroudas, C. (1992). Convergences among psychoanalytic and cognitive-behavioral theories of depression.Psychoanalytic Psychology, 9, 157–190.Google Scholar
  10. Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory.American Psychologist, 36, 129–148.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Bower, G. H. (1987). Commentary on mood and memory.Behaviour Research and Therapy, 25, 443–455.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Bowlby, J. (1988). Developmental psychiatry becomes of age.American Journal of Psychiatry, 145, 1–10.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Cohen, J. (1977).Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  14. Dalgleish, T., & Watts, F. N. (1990). Biases of attention and memory in disorders of anxiety and depression.Clinical Psychology Review, 10, 589–604.Google Scholar
  15. Eich, E. (1980). The cue-dependent nature of state-dependent retrieval.Memory and Cognition, 8, 157–173.Google Scholar
  16. Ellis, H. C., & Ashbrook, P. W. (1988). Resource allocation model of the effects of depressed mood states on memory. In K. Fiedler and J. Forgas (Eds.),Affect, cognition and social behavior. Toronto: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
  17. Ellis, H. C., & Ashbrook, P. W. (1991). The “State” of mood and memory research: A selective review. In D. Kuiken (Ed.),Mood and memory: Theory, research and applications. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
  18. Fitzgerald, J. M. (1991). Autobiographical recall and mood: A schema theory account. In D. Kuiken (Ed.),Mood and memory;Theory, research and applications. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
  19. Gerlsma, C. (1993). Parental rearing styles and psychopathology: Notes on the validity of questionnaires for recalled parental behavior. In C. Perris, W. A. Arrindell, & M. Eisemann (Eds.),Parental rearing and psychopathology. Chichester: Wiley (in press).Google Scholar
  20. Gerlsma, C., Emmelkamp, P. M. G., & Arrindell, W. A. (1990). Anxiety, depression and perception of early parenting: A meta-analysis.Clinical Psychology Review, 10, 251–277.Google Scholar
  21. Gerlsma, C., Arrindell, A., & Emmelkamp, P. M. G. (1991). Mood and memories of early parenting: Connotation of two parental rearing style questionnaires.Personality and Individual Differences, 12, 551–555.Google Scholar
  22. Gerlsma, C., Kramer, J. J. A. M., Scholing, H. A., & Emmelkamp, P. M. G. (1992).The influence of mood on memories of parental rearing practices (manuscript submitted for publication).Google Scholar
  23. Gotlib, I. H., Mount, J. H., Cordie, N. I., & Whiffen, V. E. (1988). Depression and perception of early parenting: A longitudinal investigation.British Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 24–27.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Ingram, R. E. (1991). External validity issues in mood and memory research. In D. Kuiken (Ed.),Mood and memory: Theory, research and applications. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
  25. Kihlstrom, J. F. (1991) On what does mood-dependent memory depend? In D. Kuiken (Ed.),Mood and memory;Theory, research and applications, Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
  26. Laird, J. D., Cuniff, M., Sheehan, K., Shulman, D., & Strum, G. (1991). Emotion specific effects of facial expressions on memory for life events. In D. Kuiken (Ed.),Mood and memory: Theory, research and applications. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
  27. Lewinsohn, P. M., & Rosenbaum, M. (1987). Recall of parental behavior by acute depressives, remitted depressives, and nondepressives.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 611–619.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Lewinsohn, P. M., Mischel, W., Chaplin, W., & Barton, R. (1980). Social competence and depression: The role of illusory self-perception.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 89, 203–212.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Martin, M. (1990). On the induction of mood.Clinical Psychology Review, 10, 669–697.Google Scholar
  30. Mathews, A., & Bradley, B. (1983), Mood and the self-reference bias in recall.Behaviour Research and Therapy, 21, 233–239.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Natale, M., & Hantas, M. (1982). Effect of temporary mood states on selective memory about the self.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 927–934.Google Scholar
  32. Parker, G. (1981). Parental reports of depressives: An investigation of several explanations.Journal of Affective Disorders, 3, 131–140.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Parker, G., Tupling, H., & Brown, L. B. (1979). A parental bonding instrument.British Journal of Medical Psychology, 52, 1–10.Google Scholar
  34. Riskind, J. H. (1991). Will the field ultimately need a more detailed analysis of mood-memory? Comments on Ellis and Ashbrook. In D. Kuiken (Ed.),Mood and memory;Theory, research and applications. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
  35. Salovey, P., & Singer, J. A. (1991). Mood congruency effects in recall of childhood versus recent memories. In D. Kuiken (Ed.),Mood and memory;Theory, research and applications. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
  36. Singer, J. A., & Salovey, P. (1988). Mood and memory: evaluating the network theory of affect.Clinical Psychology Review, 8, 211–251.Google Scholar
  37. Teasdale, J. D. (1983). Negative thinking in depression: Cause, effect or reciprocal relationship?Advances in Behaviour Research and Therapy, 5, 3–25.Google Scholar
  38. Velten, E. (1968). A laboratory task for induction of mood states.Behaviour Research and Therapy, 6, 473–482.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Watts, F. N., Dalgleish, T., Bourke, P., & Healy, D. (1990). Memory deficit in clinical depression: processing resources and the structure of materials.Psychological Medicine, 20, 345–349.PubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Publishing Corporation 1992

Authors and Affiliations

  • Coby Gerlsma
    • 1
  • Ineke Mosterman
    • 1
  • Sytske Buwalda
    • 1
  • Paul M. G. Emmelkamp
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of Clinical PsychologyAcademic HospitalEZ GroningenThe Netherlands

Personalised recommendations