Skip to main content
Log in

Proxemics, locus of control, anxiety, and type of movement in emotionally disturbed and normal boys

  • Published:
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In order to determine the interpersonal distancing requirements for emotionally disturbed and normal children and in order to investigate the relationship of locus of control and anxiety to interpersonal space, 20 emotionally disturbed and 20 normal boys were randomly required to approach an object person and to let the object person approach them until they felt uncomfortable. Results indicated that emotionally disturbed boys required more space than normals; that subjects would approach closer than they would allow the object person to approach them; and that externals required more space than internals. There were no significant differences between high and low anxious subjects, nor between emotionally disturbed children diagnostically classified as overanxious reaction and those with other diagnosis. Finally, neither anxiety nor locus of control explained the significant normal—emotionally disturbed differences in space requirements. Theoretical and practical implications were discussed as well as the relationship between the present and previous research.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bailey, K. G., Harnett, J. J., & Gibson, F. W., Jr. Implied threat and the territorial factor in personal space.Psychological Reports, 1972,30, 263–270.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burckhardt, D. Mowenbeobachtungen in Basel.Ornithologische Beobachter, 1944,5, 49–76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duke, M. P., & Mullens, M. C. Preferred interpersonal distance as a function of locus of control orientation in schizophrenics, nonschizophrenic patients, and normals.Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1973,41, 230–234.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duke, M. P., & Nowicki, S., Jr. A new measure and social learning model for interpersonal distance.Journal of Experimental Research in Personality, 1972,6, 1–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, R. L. Social schema of normal and disturbed school children.Journal of Educational Psychology, 1967,58, 88–92.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haase, R. F., & Markley, M. S. A methodological note on the study of personal space.Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1973,40, 122–125.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, E. T. A system for the notation of proxemic behavior.American Anthropologist, 1963,65, 1003–1026.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, E. T.The hidden dimension. New York: Doubleday, 1966.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hartnett, J. J., Bailey, K. G., & Gibson, F. W., Jr. Personal space as influenced by sex and type of movement.Journal of Psychology, 1970,76, 139–144.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hediger, H.Wild animals in captivity. London: Butterworths Scientific Publications, 1950.

    Google Scholar 

  • Horowitz, M. J., Duff, D., & Stratton, L. The body buffer zone: An exploration of personal space.Archives of General Psychiatry, 1964,11, 651–656.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keuthe, J. L. Social schemas.Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1962,64, 31–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mallenby, T. W. Personal space: Projective and direct measures with institutionalized mentally retarded children.Journal of Personality Assessment, 1974,38, 28–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • McBride, G., King, M. G., & James, S. W. Social proxemity effects of galvanic skin responses in adult humans.Journal of Psychology, 1965,61, 153–157.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mehrabian, A. Significance of posture and position in the communication of attitude and status relationships.Psychological Bulletin, 1969,71, 359–372.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newman, R. C., & Pollack, D. Proxemics in deviant adolescents.Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1973,40, 6–8.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nowicki, S., Jr., & Strickland, B. R. A locus of control scale for children.Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1973,40, 148–154.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sommer, R. Small group ecology.Psychological Bulletin, 1967,67, 145–152.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sommer, R.Personal space. Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall, 1969.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spielberger, C. D. Anxiety as an emotional state. In C. D. Spielberger (Ed.),Anxiety: Current trends in theory and research. New York: Academic Press, 1972.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spielberger, C. D.Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory of Children. Palo Alto, California: Consulting Psychologists Press, 1973.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolor, A. Psychological distance in disturbed and normal children.Psychological Reports, 1968,25, 659–701.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolor, A., Brannigan, G. G., & Murphey, V. M. Psychological distance, future time perspective, and internal-external expectancy.Journal of Projective Techniques and Personality, 1970,34, 283–294.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weinstein, L. Social schemata of emotional disturbed boys.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1965,70, 457–461.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Kendall, P.C., Deardorff, P.A., Finch, A.J. et al. Proxemics, locus of control, anxiety, and type of movement in emotionally disturbed and normal boys. J Abnorm Child Psychol 4, 9–16 (1976). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00917601

Download citation

  • Revised:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00917601

Keywords

Navigation