Skip to main content
Log in

The functional independence of response latency and accuracy: Implications for the concept of conceptual tempo

  • Published:
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Kagan (1965a) developed the concepts of impulsive and reflective cognitive styles (conceptual tempo) to add a new dimension to the understanding and assessment of human intelligence. Although latency (the principal component of conceptual tempo) is negatively correlated with academic performance, it may not be necessary to modify latency in order to modify accuracy. With 40 disadvantaged preschool children, it was found that reinforcing long latencies in choice tasks did not increase accuracy and vice versa, and that reinforcing both long latencies and accuracy was no more effective than reinforcing accuracy alone. These data were used to question the usefulness of the construct of conceptual tempo.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Cathcart, W. G., & Liedtke, W. Reflectiveness/impulsiveness and mathematics achievement.The Arithmetic Teacher, 1969(Nov.), 563–567.

  • Debus, R. L. Effects of brief observation of model behavior on conceptual tempo of impulsive children.Developmental Psychology, 1970,2, 22–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Denney, D. R. Modeling effects upon conceptual style and cognitive tempo.Child Development, 1914,43, 105–119.

    Google Scholar 

  • Egeland, B. Training impulsive children in the use of more efficient scanning techniques.Child Development, 1974,45, 165–171.

    Google Scholar 

  • Errickson, E. A., Wyne, M. D., & Routh, D. K. A response-cost procedure for reduction of impulsive behavior of academically handicapped children.Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 1973,1, 350–357.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hemry, F. P. Effect of reinforcement conditions on a discrimination learning task for impulsive versus reflective children.Child Development, 1973,44, 657–660.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kagan, J. Impulsive and reflective children: Significance of conceptual tempo. In J. D. Krumboltz (Ed.),Learning and the educational process. Chicago: Rand McNally, 1965. Pp. 133–161.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kagan, J. Reflection-impulsivity and reading ability in primary grade children.Child Development, 1965,36, 609–628.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kagan J. Reflection-impulsivity: The generality and dynamics of conceptual tempo.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1966,71(1), 17–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kagan, J., Rosmon, B. L., Day, D., Albert, and Phillips, W. Information processing in the child: Significance of analytic and reflective attitudes.Psychological Monographs, 1964, 78(1, Whole No. 578).

  • Kagan, J., Pearson, L., & Welch, L. Conceptual impulsivity and inductive reasoning.Child Development, 1966,37, 583–594.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kagan, J., Pearson, L., & Welch, L. Modifiability of an impulsive tempo.Journal of Educational Psychology, 1966,57, 359–365.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lahey, B. B., & Lefton, L. A. Discrimination of letter combinations in good and poorKaders.Journal of Special Education, 1916,10, 205–210.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lahey, B. B., & McNees, M. P. Letter discrimination errors in kindergarten through third grade: Assessment and operant training.Journal of Special Education, 1915,9, 191–199.

    Google Scholar 

  • Massari, D. J., & Schack, M. L. Discrimination learning by reflective and impulsive children as a function of reinforcement schedule.Developmental Psychology, 1972,6, 183.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meichenbaum, D. H., & Goodman, J. Training impulsive children to talk to themselves: A means of developing self control.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1971,11, 115–126.

    Google Scholar 

  • Messer, S. B. Reflection-impulsivity: Stability and school failure.Journal of Educational Psychology, 1970,61, 487–490.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ridberg, E. H., Parke, R. D., & Hetherington, E. M. Modification of impulsive and reflective cognitive styles through observation of film-mediated models.Developmental Psychology, 1971,5, 369–377.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F.The technology of teaching. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1969.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wright, J. C.The Kansas Reflection-impulsivity Scale for Preschoolers (KRISP). St. Louis: CEMREL, 1971.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zeinicker, T., Jefferey, W. E., Ault, R., & Parsons, J. Analysis and modification of search strategies of impulsive and reflective children on the Matching Familiar Figures test.Child Development, 1972,43, 321–335.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

This article is based on a master's thesis prepared by the first author under the supervision of the second author.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Williams, M., Lahey, B.B. The functional independence of response latency and accuracy: Implications for the concept of conceptual tempo. J Abnorm Child Psychol 5, 371–378 (1977). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00915085

Download citation

  • Revised:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00915085

Keywords

Navigation