Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology

, Volume 21, Issue 1, pp 103–117 | Cite as

Narrative abilities in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and normal peers

  • Rosemary Tannock
  • Karen L. Purvis
  • Russell J. Schachar
Article

Abstract

A story retelling task was used to assess narrative abilities in 30 boys with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and 30 normally developing boys, matched on age and IQ. Each boy listened to two stories and retold them for another child. Results indicated that the two groups did not differ in their ability to comprehend and extract the main ideas from the stories, but did differ in the production of narratives. Boys with ADHD provided less information overall, and their stories were more poorly organized and less cohesive and contained more inaccuracies. As a result, their stories were often confused and hard to follow. Organization and monitoring of information are functions of executive control. Thus the observed deficits in narrative production in children with ADHD may reflect underlying deficits in executive processes.

Keywords

Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Main Idea Attention Deficit Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Executive Control 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. (1987).Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed., rev.). Washington, DC: Author.Google Scholar
  2. Barkley, R. A. (1990).Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. A handbook for diagnosis and treatment (pp. 76–77). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  3. Beitchman, J. H., Hood, J., Rochon, J., Peterson, M. (1989). Empirical classification of speech/language impairment in children II. Behavioral characteristics.Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,28, 118–123.Google Scholar
  4. Benezra, E., & Douglas, V. I. (1988). Short-term serial recall in ADD-H, normal and reading disabled boys.Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 16, 511–525.Google Scholar
  5. Brown, A. L., Day, J. D., & Jones, R. S. (1983). The development of plans for summarizing texts.Child Development, 54, 968–979.Google Scholar
  6. Brown, A. L., & Smiley, S. S. (1977). Rating the importance of structural units of prose passages: A problem of metacognitive development.Child Development, 48, 1–8.Google Scholar
  7. Cazden, C. B. (1988). Classroom Discourse: The language of teaching and learning. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemon.Google Scholar
  8. Douglas, V. I., Barr, R. G., Amin, K., O'Neill, M. E., & Britton, B. G. (1988). Dosage effects and individual responsivity to methylphenidate in attention deficit disorder.Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 29, 453–475.Google Scholar
  9. Douglas, V. I., & Benezra, E. (1990). Supraspan verbal memory in attention deficit disorder with hyperactivity, normal and reading-disabled boys.Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 18, 617–638.Google Scholar
  10. Fine, J. (1985). Cohesion as an index of social-cognitive factors: Oral language of the reading disabled.Discourse Processes, 8, 91–112.Google Scholar
  11. German, D. J., & Simon, E. (1991). Analysis of childen's word-finding skills in discourse.Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 34, 309–316.Google Scholar
  12. Hamlett, K. W., Pellegrini, D. S., & Connors, C. K. (1987). An investigation of executive processes in the problem solving of attention deficit disorder-hyperactive children.Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 12, 227–240.Google Scholar
  13. Hinshaw, S. P. (1992). Externalizing behavior problems and academic underachievement in childhood and adolescence: Causal relationships and underlying mechanisms.Psychological Bulletin, 111, 127–155.Google Scholar
  14. Jastak, S., & Wilkinson, G. S. (1984).Manual for the Wide Range Achievement Test (rev.). Wilmington, DE: Jastak.Google Scholar
  15. Liles, B. Z., & Purcell, S. (1987). Departures in the spoken narratives of normal and language-disordered children.Applied Psycholinguistics, 8, 185–202.Google Scholar
  16. Love, A. J., & Thompson, M. G. (1988). Language disorders and attention deficit disorders in young children referred for psychiatric services.American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 58, 52–64.Google Scholar
  17. McGee, R., Partridge, F., Williams, S., & Silva, P. A. (1991). A twelve-year follow-up of preschool hyperactive children.Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 30, 224–232.Google Scholar
  18. Neisser, U. (1967).Cognitive psychology. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
  19. Norris, J. A. & Bruning, R. H. (1988). Cohesion in the narratives of good and poor readers.Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 53, 416–424.Google Scholar
  20. O'Neill, M. E., & Douglas, V. I. (1991). Study strategies and story recall in attention deficit disorder, reading disabled and normal boys.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 19, 671–692.Google Scholar
  21. Purvis, K. L. (1992).Narrative abilities in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, learning disabilities and normal controls. Unpublished master's thesis. University of Toronto, Ontario, Canada.Google Scholar
  22. Purvis, K. L., Schachar, R., & Tannock, R. (1992). Comparative analysis of narrative language abilities in boys with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and learning disabilities. Manuscript submitted for publication.Google Scholar
  23. Schachar, R., & Logan, G. (1990). Impulsivity and inhibitory control in normal development and childhood psychopathology.Developmental Psychology, 26, 710–720.Google Scholar
  24. Schachar, R., & Tannock, R. (1990).Teacher Telephone Interview for Disruptive Behavior Disorders (DSM-III-R). Unpublished manuscript. Hospital for Sick Children, Department of Psychiatry, Toronto.Google Scholar
  25. Schachar, R. J., & Wachsmuth, R. (1989).Parent Interview for Child Symptoms-Revised DSM-III-R. Unpublished manuscript, Hospital for Sick Children, Department of Psychiatry, Toronto.Google Scholar
  26. Smiley, S. S., Oakley, D. D., Worthen, D., Campione, J. C., & Brown, A.L. (1977). Recall of thematically relevant material by adolescent good and poor readers as a function of written versus oral presentation.Journal of Educational Psychology, 69, 381–387.Google Scholar
  27. Tannock, R., Schachar, R., Carr, R. P., Chajczyk, D., & Logan, G. D. (1989). Effects of methylphenidate on inhibitory control in hyperactive children.Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 17, 473–491.Google Scholar
  28. van der Meere, J., & Sergeant, J. (1988). Focused attention in pervasively hyperactive children.Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 16, 627–639.Google Scholar
  29. van Dijk, T., & Kintsch, W. (1983).Strategies of Discourse comprehension. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  30. Voelker, S. L., Carter, R. A., Sprague, D. J., Gdowski, C. L., & Lachar, D. (1989). Developmental trends in memory and metamemory in children with attention deficit disorder.Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 14, 75–88.Google Scholar
  31. Weingartner, H., Rapoport, J. L., Buschbaum, M. S., Bunney, W. E., Ebert, M. H., Mikkelson, E. L., & Caine, E. D. (1980). Cognitive processes in normal and hyperactive children and their response to amphetamine treatment.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 89, 25–37.Google Scholar
  32. Whalen, C. K., Henker, B., Collins, B. E., McAuliffe, S., & Vaux, A. (1979). Peer interaction in a structured communication task: Comparisons of normal and hyperactive boys and of methylphenidate (Ritalin) and placebo effects.Child Development, 50, 388–401.Google Scholar
  33. Zentall, S. S. (1988). Production deficiencies in elicited language but not in the spontaneous verbalizations of hyperactive children.Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 16, 657–673.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Publishing Corporation 1993

Authors and Affiliations

  • Rosemary Tannock
    • 1
  • Karen L. Purvis
    • 1
  • Russell J. Schachar
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of Psychiatry ResearchHospital for Sick ChildrenTorontoCanada

Personalised recommendations