Skip to main content
Log in

Stratification in perception and action

  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Summary

This paper discusses the concept of stratification, in the sense of levels of analysis and levels of control, in relation to human perception and performance. It is contended that functional analysis is the proper level of analysis for the domain of perception and action. This is illustrated by means of models of cognitive energetics and motor control. The functional level of analysis can be situated in between the level of symbolic representations and the level of neurophysiological mechanisms. It is also argued that the concept of levels of control provides a way of integrating or relating ecological and information-processing approaches to psychology, in the sense that ecological psychology is concerned with lower and more peripheral levels of control (coordinative structures). Finally, some remarks are made on the relation between cognition and perception and action, drawing on Piaget, and it is proposed that the concept of schema is more appropriate for the domain of perception and action than is the notion of symbolic representations.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abbs, H. H., & Cole, K. J. (1987). Neural mechanisms of motor equivalence and goal achievement. In S. P. Wise (Ed.),Higher brain functions. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arbib, M. A. (1987). Levels of modeling of mechanisms of visually guided behavior.Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 10, 407–465.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arbib, M. A., & Hesse, M. (1986).The construction of reality. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. (1986).Working memory. Oxford: Clarendon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bartlett, F. C. (1932).Remembering. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ben-Zeev, A. (1988). The schema paradigm in perception.Journal of Mind and Behavior, 9, 487–514.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bernstein, N. A. (1987).Bewegungsphysiologie. Leipzig: Barth.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boden, M. A. (1972).Purposive explanation in psychology. Hassocks: Harvester.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bootsma, R. (1988).The timing of rapid interceptive actions. Amsterdam: Free University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broadbent, D. E. (1977). Levels, hierarchies, and the locus of control.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 29, 181–201.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bunge, M. (1980).The mind-body problem. Oxford: Pergamon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chase, W. G. (1978). Elementary information processes. In W. K. Ester (Ed.),Handbook of learning and cognitive processes (Vol. 5),Human Information Processing, (pp. 18–89). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Churchland, P. M. (1981). Eliminative materialism and the propositional attitudes.Journal of Philosophy, 78, 67–90.

    Google Scholar 

  • Churchland, P. S. (1986).Neurophilosophy. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cummins, R. (1975). Functional analysis.Journal of Philosophy, 72, 741–760.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cummins, R. (1983).The nature of psychological explanation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cummins, R. (1989).Meaning and mental representation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dewey, J. (1896). The reflex arc concept in psychology.Psychological Review, 3, 352–370.

    Google Scholar 

  • Epstein, W. (1986). Contrasting conceptions of perception and action.Acta Psychologica, 63, 103–115.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. A. (1975).The language of thought. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. A. (1980). Methodological solipsism considered as a research strategy in cognitive psychology.Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 3, 63–109.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. A. (1981). The mind-body problem.Scientific American, 244, 1, 124–132.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. A. (1984).The modularity of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. A. (1985). Precis of The Modularity of Mind.Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 8, 1–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. A. (1987).Psychosemantics. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fowler, C. A., & Turvey, M. T. (1982). Observational perspective and descriptive level in perceiving and acting. In W. B. Weimer & D. S. Palermo (Ed.),Cognition and the symbolic processes (p. 1–19). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gatlistel, C. R. (1980).The organisation of action. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gardner, H. (1985).The mind's new science. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, E. J. (1982). The concept of affordances in development: The renascence of functionalism. In W. A. Collins (Ed.),The concept of development. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J. J. (1979).The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gopher, D., & Sanders, A. F. (1984). S-Oh-R: Oh Stages! Oh Resources! In W. Prinz & A. F. Sanders (Eds.),Cognition and motor processes (pp. 231–253). Berlin, Heidelberg, New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Granit, R. (1977).The purposive brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gray, J. (1987). The mind-brain identity theory as a scientific hypothesis: A second look. In C. Blakemoore & S. Greenfield (Eds.),Mindwaves: Thoughts on intelligence, identity and consciousness (pp. 461–483). Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hebb, D. O. (1949).The organisation of behavior. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hebb, D. O. (1955). Drives and the C.N.S. (conceptual nervous system).Psychological Review, 62, 243–254.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Holst, E. von (1937). On the nature of order in the central nervous system. In C. R. Gallistel (Ed.),The organisation of action (pp. 81–107). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum 1980.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holst, E. von, & Mittelstaedt, H. (1950). The reafference principle. In C. R. Gallistel (Ed.),The organisation of action (pp. 176–209). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum 1980.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. (1943).Principles of behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jeannerod, M. (1988).The neural and behavioral organisation of goaldirected movements. Oxford: Clarendon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D. (1973).Attention and effort. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • LaBerge, D., & Samuels, S. J. (1974). Toward a theory of automatic information processing in reading.Cognitive Psychology, 6, 292–323.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lashley, K. (1951). The problem of serial order in behavior. In L. A. Jeffres (Ed.),Cerebral mechanisms in behavior. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luria, A. R. (1973).The working brain. Harmondsworth: Penguin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lynch, J. C. (1980). The functional organisation of the posterior parietal association cortex.Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 3, 485–534.

    Google Scholar 

  • Matthen, M. (1988). Biological functions and perceptual content.Journal of Philosophy, 85, 5–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meijer, O. G., & Roth, K. (Eds.), (1988).Complex Movement Behavior: The motor action controversy. Amsterdam: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mounteastle, V. B. (1986). The neural mechanisms of cognitive functions can now be studied directly.Trends in Neurosciences, 9, 505–508.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neisser, U. (1976).Cognition and reality. San Francisco: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neumann, O. (1984). Automatic processing: A review of recent findings and a plea for an old theory. In W. Prinz & A. F. Sanders (Eds.),Cognition and motor processes (pp. 225–294). Berlin, Heidelberg, New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newell, A. (1980). Physical symbol systems.Cognitive Science, 4, 135–183.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oldfield, R. C., & Zangwill, O. L. (1942 – 1943). Head's concept of the schema and its application in contemporary British psychology (I – IV),British Journal of Psychology, 32 – 33.

  • Pattee, H. H. (1973). The physical basis and origin of hierarchical control. In H. H. Pattee (Ed.),Hierarchy Theory. The challenge of complex systems (pp. 73–108). New York: Braziller.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pew, R. W. (1984). A distributed processing view of human motor control. In W. Prinz & A. E. Sanders (Eds.),Cognition and motor processes (pp. 19–28). Berlin, Heidelberg, New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1967).Biologie et connaissance. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I. (1978).Chronometric explorations of mind. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I., & Keele, S. W. (1968). On the genesis of abstract ideas.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 77, 353–363.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I., & Snyder, C. C. R. (1975). Facilitation and inhibition in the processing of signals. In P. M. A. Rabbitt & S. Dornic (Eds.),Attention and Performance: V (pp. 669–682). London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pribram, K. H., & McGuinness, D. (1975). Arousal, activation and effort in the control of attention.Psychological Review, 82, 116–149.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Putnam, H. (1980). The nature of mental states. In N. Block (Ed.),Readings in the philosophy of psychology (Vol 1, pp. 221–231). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Putnam, H. (1981). Reductionism and the nature of psychology. In J. Haugeland (Ed.),Mind design (pp. 205–219). Montgomery: Bradford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Putnam, H. (1988).Representation and reality. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Quine, W. V. O. (1962). Two dogmas of empiricism. In W. V. O. Quine (Ed.),From a logical point of view (pp. 20–46). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rabbitt, P. (1984). The control of attention in visual search. In R. Parasuraman & D. R. Davies (Eds.),Varieties of attention (pp. 273–291). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rabbitt, E (1986). Models and paradigms in the study of stress effects. In G. R. J. Hockey, A. W. K. Gailard, & M. G. H. Coles (Eds.),Energetics and human information processing (pp. 155–174). Dordrecht: Nijhof.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reed, E. S. (1984). From action gestalts to direct action. In H. T. A. Whiting (Ed.),Human motor actions: Bernstein reassessed (pp. 157–170). Amsterdam: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reed, E. S. (1988). Applying the theory of action systems to the study of motor skills. In O. G. Meijer & K. Roth (Eds.),Complex movement behavior: The motor action controversy. Amsterdam: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sanders, A. F. (1980). Stage analysis of reaction processes. In G. E. Stelmach & J. Requin (Eds.),Tutorials in motor behavior (pp. 331–354). Amsterdam: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sanders, A. F. (1983). Towards a model of stress and human performance.Acta Psychologica, 53, 61–97.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sanders, C., & Rappard, H. V. (1985). Psychology and philosophy of science. In K. B. Madsen & L. P. Mos (Eds.),Annals of Theoretical Psychology (pp. 219–268). New York, London: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scheerer, E. (1984). Motor theories of cognitive structure: A historical review. In W. Prinz & A. F. Sanders (Eds.),Cognition and motor processes (pp. 77–98). Berlin, Heidelberg, New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, R. A. (1975). A schema theory of discrete motor learning.Psychological Review, 82, 225–260.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W. (1985). Toward a model of attention and the development of automatic processing. In M. I. Posner & O. S. M. Marin (Eds.),Attention and performance: 11 (pp. 475–492). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shepard, R. N. (1984). Ecological constraints on internal representation: Resonant kinematics of perceiving, imagining, thinking, and dreaming.Psychological Review, 91, 417–447.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shiffrin, R. M., & Schneider, W. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending and a general theory.Psychological Review, 84, 127–189.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stelmach, G. E., & Diggles, V. A. (1982). Control theories in motor behavior.Acta Psychologica, 50, 83–105.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stuss, D. T., & Benson, D. F. (1986).The frontal lobes. New York: Raven Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Toulmin, S. (1972). The mentality of man's brain. In A. G. Karczmar & J. C. Eccles (Eds.),Brain and human behavior. Berlin, Heidelberg, New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turvey, M. T. (1977). Preliminaries to a theory of action with reference to vision. In R. Shaw & J. Bransford (Eds.),Perceiving, acting, knowing (pp. 24–265). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turvey, M. T., & Carello, C. (1981). Cognition: The view from ecological realism.Cognition, 10, 313–321.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weimer, W. B. (1977). A conceptual framework for cognitive psychology: Motor theories of the mind. In R. Shaw & J. Bransford (Eds.),Perceiving, acting, knowing (pp. 267–311). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weiss, P. (1941). Self-differentiation of the basic patterns of co-ordination.Comparative Psychology Monographs,12 (4).

  • Wilkes, K. V. (1978).Physicalism. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilkes, K. V. (1980). Brain states.British Journal for the Philosophy of Sience, 31, 111–129.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilkes, K. V. (1982). Functionalism, psychology and the philosophy of mind. In J. I. Biro & R. W. Shahan (Eds.),Mind, brain, and function (pp. 146–168). Brighton: Harvester.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wimsatt, W. C. (1976). Reductionism, levels of organisation, and the mind-body problem. In C. G. Globus, G. Maxwell, & I. Savodnik (Eds.),Consciousness and the brain (pp. 205–267). New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wright, L. (1973). Functions.Philosophical Review, 82, 139–168.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Looren de Jong, H., Sanders, A.F. Stratification in perception and action. Psychol. Res 52, 216–228 (1990). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00877530

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00877530

Keywords

Navigation