Skip to main content
Log in

Implicit and explicit memory: Implications for the symbol-manipulation versus connectionism controversy

  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Summary

This paper deals with three issues. First, the symbol-manipulation versus connectionism controversy is discussed briefly. Both can be seen as formalisms or languages for describing human behavior, but it is argued that these languages are not fully equivalent. The importance of the ability of connectionist models to learn autonomously, often underestimated, allows them to perform tasks that may defy formalization. Secondly, the structural and functional characteristics of a new connectionist learning model are presented. It avoids some of the psychological and biological implausibilities of currently popular models and solves some of the problems and shortcomings of these models. Thirdly, some simulation results are presented. The model successfully simulates the dissociation between explicit and implicit memory performance as found in patients with anterograde amnesia. This simulation provides an example of the advantages of using the connectionist language in the domain of memory psychology. It is also a good example of how psychological evidence can be used to improve connectionist models.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bechtel, W. (1985). Contemporary connectionism: Are the new parallel distributed processing models cognitive or associationist?Behaviorism, 13, 53–61.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, G. A., & Grossberg, S. (1988). Neural dynamics of category learning and recognition: Attention, memory consolidation, and amnesia. In J. L. Davis, R. W. Newburgh, E. J. Wegman (Eds.),Brain structure, learning, and memory Boulder, Co.: Westview Press, AAAS Symposium series.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cleeremans, A., Servan-Schreiber, D., & McClelland, J. L. (1989). Finite state automata and simple recurrent networks.Neural Computation, 1, 372–381.

    Google Scholar 

  • Craik, F. I. M., & Lockhart, R. S. (1972). Levels of processing: A framework for memory research.Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 671–684.

    Google Scholar 

  • Creutzfeldt, O. D. (1977). Generality of the functional structure of the neocortex.Naturwissenschaften, 64, 507–517.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Crick, F. H. C. (1979). Thinking about the brain.Scientific American, 241, 181–188.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennett, D. (1979).Brainstorms: Philosophical essays on mind and psychology. Sussex: Harverster Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elman, J. L. (1989). Structured representations and connectionist models.Proceedings of the 11th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Hillsdale, NJ: ErIbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, I. A., & Ballard, D. M. (1982). Connectionist models and their properties.Cognitive Science, 6, 205–254.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graf, P., & Mandler, G. (1984). Activation makes words more accessible, but not necessarily more retrievable.Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 23, 553–568.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hopfield, J. J., & Tank, D. W. (1986). Computing with neural circuits: A model.Science, 233, 625–633.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jordan, M. I. (1986). Serial order: A parallel processing approach. Technical Report, Institute for Cognitive Science, University of California, San Diego.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kihlstrom, J. F. (1987). The cognitive unconscious.Science, 237, 1445–1452.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kuperstein, M. (1988). Neural model of adaptive hand-eye coordination for single postures.Science, 239, 1308–1311.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Levelt, W. J. M. (1989). De connectionistische mode: Symbolische en subsymbolische modellen van her menselijk gedrag. In C. Brown, P. Hagoort, & T. Meijering (Eds.),Vensters op de geest, (pp. 202–219). Utrecht: Stichting Grafiet.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lorenz, E. N. (1963). Deterministic nonperiodic flow.Journal of Atmospheric Science, 20, 130–141.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maas, H. J. L., van der, Verschure, P. F. M. J., & Molenaar, P. C. M. (1990). A note on chaotic behavior in simple neural networks.Neural Networks, (in press).

  • Marshall, J. C. (1989). An open mind?Nature, 339, 25–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Massaro, D. W. (1988). Some criticism of connectionist models of human performance.Journal of Memory and Language, 27, 213–234.

    Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, J. L., & Rumelhart, D. E. (Eds.) (1986).Parallel distributed processing (Vol. 2). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murre, J. M. J., Phaf, R. H., & Wolters, G. (1989). CALM networks: A modular approach to supervised and unsupervised learning.IEEE-INNS, Proceedings of the International Joint Conference on Neural Networks, June 1989, Vol. 1, pp. 649 – 656.

  • Murre, J. M. J., Phaf., R. H., & Wolters, G. (in preparation). CALM: Categorizing and learning module.

  • Phaf, R. H., & Murre, J. M. J. (1989). Cognitive onder de microscop. In C. Brown, P. Hagoort, & T. Meijering (Eds.),Vensters op de geest, (pp. 164–201). Utrecht: Stichting Grafiet.

    Google Scholar 

  • Phaf, R. H., Posrma, E. O., & Wolters, G. (1990). ELAN-1: A connectionist model for implicit and explicit memory tasks. In preparation.

  • Prince, A., & Pinker, S. (1988). Rules and connections in human language.TINS, 11, 195–202.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Raaijmakers, J. G. W., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1981). Search of associative memory.Psychological Review, 88, 93–134.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reilly, R. (1989). On the relationship between connectionism and cognitive science.Irish Journal of Psychology, 10, 162–187.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richardson-Klavehn, A., & Bjork, R. A. (1988). Measures of memory.Annual Review of Psychology, 39, 474–543.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roorda-Hrdlickova, V., Wolters, G., Bonke, B., & Phaf, R. H. (1990). Unconscious perception during general anaesthesia demonstrated by an implicit memory task. In B. Bonke, W. Fitch, K. Millar (Eds.),Memory and awareness in anaesthesia (pp. 150–155). Amsterdam: Swets & Zeitlinger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D. E., & McClelland, J. L. (Eds.) (1986a).Parallel distributed processing (Vol. 1). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D. E., & McClelland, J. L. (1986b.). On learning the past tenses of English verbs. In J. L. McClelland, & D. E. Rumelhart (Eds.),Parallel distributed processing (Vol. 2, pp. 216–271). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schacter, D. L. (1987). Implicit memory: History and current status.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 13, 501–518.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skarda, C. A., & Freeman, W. J. (1987). How brains make chaos to make sense of the world.Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 10, 161–195.

    Google Scholar 

  • Squire, L. R., & Cohen, N. J. (1984). Human memory and amnesia. In J. McGaugh, G. Lynch, & N. Weinberger (Eds.),Proceedings of the conference on the neurobiology of learning and memory (pp. 3–64). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Szentágothai, J. (1975). The “module-concept” in cerebral cortext architecture.Brain Research, 95, 475–496.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Warrington, E. K., & Weiskrantz, L. (1970). Amnesia: Consolidation or retrieval.Nature, 217, 972–974.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolters, G. (1984). Memory: Two systems or one system with many subsystems?Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 7, 256–257.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

The second author is supported by the Dutch Organization for Scientific Research (NWO)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Wolters, G., Phaf, R.H. Implicit and explicit memory: Implications for the symbol-manipulation versus connectionism controversy. Psychol. Res 52, 137–144 (1990). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00877521

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00877521

Keywords

Navigation