Allport, D. A. (1987). Selection for action: Some behavioral and neurophysiological considerations of attention and action. In H. Heuer & A. F. Sanders (Eds.),Perspectives on perception and action. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Google Scholar
Allport, D. A. (1989). Visual attention. In M. I. Posner (Ed.),Foundations of cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Google Scholar
Bongartz, W., & Scheerer, E. (1976). Two visual stores and two processing operations in tachistoscopic partial report.Quarterly Journal ofExperimental Psychology, 28, 203–219.
Google Scholar
Broadbent, D. E. (1958).Perception and communication. London: Pergamon Press.
Google Scholar
Broadbent, D. E. (1971).Decision and stress. London: Academic Press.
Google Scholar
Broadbent, D. E. (1982). Task combination and selective intake of information.Acta Psychologica, 50, 253–290.
Google Scholar
Broadbent, D. E., & Broadbent, M. H. P. (1986). Encoding speed of visual features and the occurrence of illusory conjunctions.Perception, 15, 515–524.
Google Scholar
Broadbent, D. E., & Broadbent, M. H. P. (1987). From detection to identification: Response to multiple targets in rapid serial visual presentation.Perception & Psychophysics, 42, 105–113.
Google Scholar
Bundesen, C. (1990). A theory of visual attention.Psychological Review, 97, 523–547.
Google Scholar
Bundesen, C. (1991). Visual selection of features and objects: Is location special? A reinterpretation of Nissen's (1985) findings.Perception & Psychophysics, 50, 87–89.
Google Scholar
Bundesen, C., Pedersen, L. F., & Larsen, A. (1984). Measuring efficiency of selection from briefly exposed visual displays: A model for partial report.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 329–339.
Google Scholar
Bundesen, C., Shibuya, H., & Larsen, A. (1985). Visual selection from multielement displays: A model for partial report. In M. I. Posner & O. S. M. Marin (Eds.),Attention and performance XI. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Google Scholar
Butler, B. E., Mewhort, D. J. K., & Tramer, S. C. (1987). Location errors in tachistoscopic recognition: Guesses, probe errors, or spatial confusions?Canadian Journal ofPsychology, 41, 339–350.
Google Scholar
Cave, K. R., & Wolfe, J. M. (1990). Modeling the role of parallel processing in visual search.Cognitive Psychology, 22, 225–271.
Google Scholar
Clark, S. E. (1969). Retrieval of color information from preperceptual memory.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 82, 263–266.
Google Scholar
DeYoe, E. A., & Van Essen, D. C. (1988). Concurrent processing streams in monkey visual cortex.Trends in Neuro Sciences, 11, 219–226.
Google Scholar
Driver, J., & Baylis, G. C. (1989). Movement and visual attention: The spotlight metaphor breaks down.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15, 448–456.
Google Scholar
Driver, J., & Halligan, P. W. (1991). Can visual neglect operate in object-centred co-ordinates? An affirmitive single-case study.Cognitive Neuropsychology, 8, 475–496.
Google Scholar
Duncan, J. (1980). The demonstration of capacity limitation.Cognitive Psychology, 12, 75–96.
Google Scholar
Duncan, J. (1981). Directing attention in the visual field.Perception & Psychophysics, 30, 90–93.
Google Scholar
Duncan, J. (1984). Selective attention and the organization of visual information.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113, 501–517.
Google Scholar
Duncan, J., & Humphreys, G. W. (1989). Visual search and stimulus similarity.Psychological Review, 96, 433–458.
Google Scholar
Eimer, M., Nattkemper, D., Schrbger, E., & Prinz, W. (in press). Unwillkürliche Aufmerksamkeit. In O. Neumann & A. F. Sanders (Eds.), Aufmerksamkeit. Göttingen, Hogrefe.
Enns, J. T. (1990). Three-dimensional features that pop out in visual search. In D. Brogan (Ed.),Visual search. London: Taylor & Francis.
Google Scholar
Enns, J. T., & Rensink, R. A. (1991). Preattentive recovery of three-dimensional orientation from line drawings.Psychological Review, 98, 335–351.
Google Scholar
Eriksen, C. W. (1990). Attentional search of the visual field. In D. Brogan (Ed.),Visual search. London: Taylor & Francis.
Google Scholar
Eriksen, C. W., & Hoffman, J. E. (1973). The extent of processing of noise elements during selective encoding from visual displays.Perception & Psychophysics, 14, 155–160.
Google Scholar
Eriksen, C. W., & Hoffman, J. E. (1974). Selective attention: Noise suppression or signal enhancement?Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 4, 587–589.
Google Scholar
Eriksen, C. W., & Rohrbaugh, J. W. (1970). Some factors determining efficiency of selective attention.American Journal of Psychology, 83, 330–343.
Google Scholar
Eriksen, C. W., & St James, J. D. (1986). Visual attention within and around the field of focal attention: A zoomlens model.Perception & Psychophysics, 40, 225–240.
Google Scholar
Eriksen, C. W., & Yeh, Y. Y. (1985). Allocation of attention in the visual field.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 5, 583–597.
Google Scholar
Felleman, D. J., & Van Essen, D. C. (1991). Distributed hierarchical processing in the primate cerebral cortex.Cerebral Cortex, 1, 1–47.
Google Scholar
Francolini, C. N., & Egeth, H. E. (1980). On the non-automaticity of “automatic” activation: Evidence of selective seeing.Perception & Psychophysics, 27, 331–342.
Google Scholar
Fryklund, I. (1975). Effects of cued-set spatial arrangement and targetbackground similarity in the partial-report paradigm.Perception & Psychophysics, 17, 375–386.
Google Scholar
Garner, W. R. (1987). Location and color as cuing dimensions in contingent classification.Perception & Psychophysics, 41, 202–210.
Google Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., & Broadbent, D. E. (1984). Combining attributes in specified and categorized target search: Further evidence for strategy differences.Memory & Cognition, 12, 329–337.
Google Scholar
Hagenzieker, M. P., Van der Heijden, A. H. C., & Hagenaar, R. (1990). Time courses in visual information processing: Some empirical evidence for inhibition.Psychological Research, 52, 13–21.
Google Scholar
Hardin, C. L. (1988).Color for philosophers. Indianapolis: Hacket.
Google Scholar
Harms, L., & Bundesen, C. (1983). Color segregation and selective attention in a nonsearch task.Perception & Psychophysics, 33, 11–19.
Google Scholar
Humphreys, G. W. (1981). Flexibility of attention between stimulus dimensions.Perception & Psychophysics, 30, 291–302.
Google Scholar
James, W. (1890/1950).The principles of psychology (Vol. 1). Authorized edition, New York: Dover Publications, Inc.
Google Scholar
Jonides, J. (1983). Further toward a model of the mind's eye's movements.Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 21, 247–250.
Google Scholar
Jonides, J., & Yantis, S. (1988). Uniqueness of abrupt visual onset in capturing attention.Perception & Psychophysics, 43, 346–354.
Google Scholar
Kahneman, D. (1973).Attention and effort. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Google Scholar
Kahneman, D., & Chajczyk, D. (1983). Tests of the automaticity of reading: Dilution of Stroop effects by color-irrelevant stimuli.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 9, 497–509.
Google Scholar
Kahneman, D., & Henik, A. (1977). Effects of visual grouping on immediate recall and selective attention. In S. Dornic (Ed.),Attention and performance VII. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Google Scholar
Kahneman, D., & Henik, A. (1981). Perceptual organization and attention. In M. Kubovy & J. R. Pomerantz (Eds.).Perceptual organization. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Google Scholar
Kahneman, D., & Treisman, A. (1984). Changing views of attention and automaticity. In R. Parasuraman and P. R. Davies (Eds.).Varieties of attention. New York: Academic Press.
Google Scholar
Kanwisher, N., & Driver, J. (1992). Objects, attributes, and visual attention: Which, what, and where.Current Directions in Psychological Science, 1, 26–31.
Google Scholar
Keele, S. W., Cohen, A., Ivry, R., Liotti, M., & Yee, P. (1988). Tests of a temporal theory of attentional binding.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14, 444–452.
Google Scholar
Kmmhansl, C. L. (1982). Abrupt changes in visual stimulation enhance processing of form and location information.Perception & Psychophysics, 32(6), 511–523.
Google Scholar
LaBerge, D., & Brown, V. (1989). Theory of attentional operations in shape identification.Psychological Review, 96, 101–124.
Google Scholar
Lawrence, D. H. (1971). Two studies of visual search for word targets with controlled rates of presentation.Perception & Psychophysics, 10, 85–89.
Google Scholar
Livingstone, M., & Hubel, D. (1988). Segregation of form, color, movement, and depth: Anatomy, physiology, and perception.Science, 240, 740–749.
Google Scholar
McClean, J. P., Broadbent, D. E., & Broadbent, M. H. P. (1982). Combining attributes in rapid serial visual presentation tasks.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 35 A, 171–186.
Google Scholar
McLeod, P., Driver, J., & Crisp, J. (1988). Visual search for a conjunction of movement and form is parallel.Nature, 332, 154–155.
Google Scholar
McLeod, P., Driver, J., Dienes, Z., & Crisp, J. (1991). Filtering by movement in visual search.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 17, 55–64.
Google Scholar
Merikle, P. M. (1980). Selection from visual persistence by perceptual groups and category membership.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 109, 279–295.
Google Scholar
Mewhort, D. J. K., Campbell, A. J., Marchetti, F. M., & Campbell, J.I.D. (1981). Identification, localisation, and “iconic memory”: An evaluation of the bar-probe task.Memory & Cognition, 9, 50–67.
Google Scholar
Miller, J. (1989). The control of attention by abrupt visual onsets and offsets.Perception & Psychophysics, 45, 567–571.
Google Scholar
Müller, H. J., & Rabbit, P. M. A. (1989). Spatial cueing and the relation between the accuracy of “Where” and “What” decisions in visual search.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 41 A, 747–773.
Google Scholar
Nakayama, K., & Mackeben, M. (1989). Sustained and transient components of focal visual attention.Vision Research, 29, 1631–1647.
Google Scholar
Nakayama, K., & Silverman, G. H. (1986). Serial and parallel processing of visual feature conjunctions.Nature, 320, 264–265.
Google Scholar
Neisser, U. (1967).Cognitive psychology. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Google Scholar
Neumann, O. (1987). Beyond capacity: A functional view of attention. In H. Heuer & A. F. Sanders (Eds.),Perspectives on perception and action. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Google Scholar
Nissen, M. J. (1985). Accessing features and objects: Is location special? In M. I. Posner & O. S. M. Marin (Eds.),Attention and performance XI. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Google Scholar
Norman, D. A. (1968). Towards a theory of memory and attention.Psychological Review, 75, 522–536.
Google Scholar
Phaf, R. H., Van der Heijden, A. H. C., and Hudson, P. T. W. (1990). SLAM: A connectionist model for attention in visual selection tasks.Cognitive Psychology, 22, 273–341.
Google Scholar
Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. The VIIth Sir Frederic Bartlett Lecture:Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32, 3–25.
Google Scholar
Posner, M. I., & Cohen, Y. (1984). Components of visual orienting. In H. Bouma & D. G. Bouwhuis (Eds.),Attention and performance X. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Google Scholar
Posner, M. I., Snyder, C. R. R., & Davidson, B. J. (1980). Attention and the detection of signals.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 109, 160–174.
Google Scholar
Prinzmetal, W., & Keysar, B. (1989). Functional theory of illusory conjunctions and neon colors.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 118, 165–190.
Google Scholar
Remington, R., & Pierce, L. (1984). Moving attention: Evidence for time-invariant shifts of visual selective attention.Perception & Psychophysics, 35, 393–399.
Google Scholar
Shapley, R. (1990). Visual sensitivity and parallel retinocortical channels.Annual Review of Psychology, 41, 635–658.
Google Scholar
Snyder, C. R. R. (1972). Selection, inspection and naming in visual search.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 92, 428–431.
Google Scholar
Sperling, G. (1960). The information available in brief visual presentations.Psychological Monograph, 74(11), (whole no. 498).
Theeuwes, J. (1991a). Cross-dimensional perceptual selectivity.Perception & Psychophysics, 50, 184–193.
Google Scholar
Theeuwes, J. (1991b). Exogeneous and endogeneous control of attention: The effect of visual onsets and offsets.Perception & Psychophysics, 49, 83–90.
Google Scholar
Theeuwes, J. (1992). Perceptual selectivity for color and form.Perception & Psychophysics, 51, 599–606.
Google Scholar
Titchener, E. B. (1908).Lectures on the elementary psychology of feeling and attention. New York: MacMillan.
Google Scholar
Todd, J. T., & Van Gelder, P. (1979). Implications of a sustained-transient dichotomy for the measurement of human performance.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 5, 625–638.
Google Scholar
Treisman, A. M. (1988). Features and objects: The fourteenth Bartlett memorial lecture.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 40A, 201–237.
Google Scholar
Treisman, A. M. (1990a). Variations on the theme of feature integration: Reply to Navon (1990).Psychological Review, 97, 460–463.
Google Scholar
Treisman, A. M. (1990b). Search and the detection and integration of features.Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 13, 454–455.
Google Scholar
Treisman, A. M., & Gelade, G. (1980). A feature integration theory of attention.Cognitive Psychology, 12, 97–136.
Google Scholar
Treisman, A. M., & Gormican, S. (1988). Feature analysis in early vision: Evidence from search asymmetries.Psychological Review, 95, 15–48.
Google Scholar
Treisman, A. M., Kahneman, D., & Burkell, J. (1983). Perceptual objects and the cost of filtering.Perception & Psychophysics, 33, 527–532.
Google Scholar
Treisman, A. M., & Sato, S. (1990). Conjunction search revisited.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16, 459–478.
Google Scholar
Tsal, Y., & Lavie, N. (1988). Attending to color and shape: The special role of location in selective visual processing.Perception & Psychophysics, 44, 15–21.
Google Scholar
Vaina, L. M. (1990). “What” and “where” in the human visual system: Two hierarchies of visual modules.Synthese, 83, 49–91.
Google Scholar
Van der Heijden, A. H. C. (1992).Selective attention in vision. London-New York: Routledge.
Google Scholar
Van der Velde, F., & Van der Heijden, A. H. C. (in press). An element in the visual field is just a conjunction of attributes: A critique of Bundesen (1991).Perception & Psychophysics.
Von Wright, J. M. (1968). Selection in visual immediate memory.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 20, 62–68.
Google Scholar
Von Wright, J. M. (1970). On selection in visual immediate memory.Acta Psychologica, 33, 280–292.
Google Scholar
Von Wright, J. M. (1972). On the problem of selection in iconic memory.Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 13, 159–171.
Google Scholar
Wolfe, J. M., Cave, K. R., & Franzel, S. L. (1989). Guided search: An alternative to the feature integration model for visual search.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15, 419–433.
Google Scholar
Yantis, S., & Jonides, J. (1984). Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: Evidence from visual search.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 601–621.
Google Scholar
Yantis, S., & Jonides, J. (1990). Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: Voluntary versus automatic allocation.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16, 121–134.
Google Scholar
Zeki, S., & Shipp, S. (1988). The functional logic of cortical connections.Nature, 355, 311–317.
Google Scholar