Skip to main content
Log in

Unconscious influences on direct-test performance

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

From intense interest in implicit memory there have evolved various methods for separating the respective influence of implicit (unconscious) and explicit (conscious) processes on performance of various tasks. Two experiments are reported, utilizing a levels-of-processing (LOP) approach to manipulate encoding level and comparable indirect (word-stem completion) and direct (cued-completion) retention tests. Confidence ratings of recollection were taken for each direct test response. The aim of these experiments was to explore the role that guessing plays in direct-test performance (Experiments 1 and 2) and to contrast this with performance in a comparable indirect test (Experiment 2). Analysis of correctly guessed responses showed that direct-test performance was reliably influenced by unconscious processes, but differently as a function of LOP. Guessing stems of nonsemantically processed words was found to enhance performance, whereas guessing stems of semantically processed words had no effect on performance. Results are discussed in terms of the similarity between guessing in a direct test and engaging in an indirect test, and subjects' unwitting resourcefulness at being able to retrieve words they cannot explicitly remember.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bowers, J. S., & Schacter, D. L. (1990). Implicit memory and test awareness.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 404–416.

    Google Scholar 

  • Challis, B. H., & Brodbeck, D. R. (1992). Level of processing affects priming in word fragment completion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition,18, 595–607.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, M. (1981). The MRC Psycholinguistic Database.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 33A, 497–505.

    Google Scholar 

  • Craik, F. I. M., & Lockhart, R. S. (1972). Levels of processing: A framework for memory research.Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 671–684.

    Google Scholar 

  • Craik, F. I. M., & Tulving, E. (1975). Depth of processing and the retention of words in episodic memory.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 104, 268–294.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dixon, N. F. (1971). Subliminal perception: The nature of a controversy. London: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gabrieli, J. D. E., Milberg, W., Keane, M. W., & Corkin, S. (1990). Intact priming of patterns despite impaired memory.Neuropsychologia, 28, 417–428.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gardiner, J. M. (1988). Functional aspects of recollectioe experience.Memory & Cognition, 16, 309–313.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graf, P., & Mandler, G. (1984). Activation makes words more accessible, but not necessarily more retrievable.Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 23, 553–568.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graf, P., Mandler, G., & Haden, P. E. (1982). Simulating amnesic symptoms in normal subjects. Science, 218, 1243–1244.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graf, P., & Schacter, D. L. (1985). Implicit and explicit memory for new associations in normal and amnesic patients.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 11, 501–518.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graf, P., Squire, L. R., & Mandler, G. (1984). The information that amnesic patients do not forget.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10, 164–178.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G. (1992). New Look 3: Unconscious cognition reclaimed.American Psychologist, 47, 766–779.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hashtroudi, S., Ferguson, S. A., Rappold, V. A., & Chrosniak, L. D. (1988). Data-driven and conceptually driven processes in partialword identification and recognition.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 14, 749–757.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holender, D. (1986). Semantic activation without conscious identification in dichotic listening, parafoveal vision, and visual masking: A survey and appraisal.Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 9, 1–66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jocoby, L. L. (1991). A process dissociation framework: Separating automatic from intentional uses of memory.Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 513–541.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jacoby, L. L., & Dallas, M. (1981). On the relationship between autobiographical memory and perceptual learning.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 110, 306–340.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jocoby, L. L., Lindsay, D. S.; & Toth, J. P. (1992). Unconscious influences revealed: Attention, awareness, and control.American Psychologist, 47, 802–809.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jocoby, L. L., Toth, J. P., & Yonelinas, A. P. (1993). Separating conscious and unconscious influences of memory: Measuring recollection.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122, 139–154.

    Google Scholar 

  • Joordens, S., & Merikle, P. M. (1993). Independence or redundancy? Two models of conscious and unconscious influences.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122, 462–467.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kihlstrom, J. F. (1987). The cognitive unconscious.Science, 237, 1445–1452.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kucera, M., & Francis, W. (1967). Computational analysis of present-day American English. Providence, RI: Brown University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lhermitte, F., & Serdaru, M. (1993). Unconscious processing in memory recall: A study of three amnesic patients.Cortex, 29, 25–43.

    Google Scholar 

  • Light, L. L., & Singh, A. (1987). Implicit and explicit memory in young and older adults.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13, 531–541.

    Google Scholar 

  • Merikle, P. M. (1992). Perception without awareness: Critical issues.American Psychologist, 47, 792–795.

    Google Scholar 

  • Merikle, P. M., & Reingold, E. M. (1991). Comparing direct (explicit) and indirect (implicit) measures to study unconscious memory.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 224–233.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, D. L., Schreiber, T. A., & McEvoy, C. L. (1992). Processing implicit and explicit representations.Psychological Review, 99, 322–348.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ornstein, R. E. (1991). The evolution of consciousness: Of Darwin, Freud, and cranial fire: The origins of the way we think. New York: Prentice Hall Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reber, A. S. (1990). The primacy of the implicit: A comment on Perruchet and Pacteau.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 119, 340–342.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reber, A. S. (1993).Implicit learning and tacit knowledge: An essay on the cognitive unconscious. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richardson-Klavehn, A., & Bjork, R. A. (1988). Measures of memory.Annual Review of Psychology, 39, 475–543.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roediger, H. L. (1990). Implicit memory: Retention without remembering.American Psychologist, 45, 1043–1056.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roediger, H. L., & McDermott, K. B. (1993). Implicit memory in normal human subjects. In F. Boller and J. Grafman (Eds.),Handbook of neuropsychology, Vol. 8 (pp. 63–131). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roediger, H. L., Weldon, M. S., Stadler, M. L., & Riegler, G. L. (1992). Direct comparison of two implicit memory tests: Word fragment and word stem completion.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 1251–1269.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schacter, D. L. (1987). Implict memory: History and current status.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13, 501–518.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schacter, D. L., Bowers, J., & Booker, J. (1989). Intention, awareness, and implicit memory: The retrieval intentionality criterion. In S. Lewandowsky, J. C. Dunn, & K. Kirsner (Eds.), Implicit memory: Theoretical issues (pp. 47–65). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schacter, D. L., Chin, C.-Y. P., & Ochsner, K. N. (1993). Implicit Memory: A selective review.Annual Review of Neuroscience, 16, 159–182.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schacter, D. L., & McGlynn, S. (1989). Implicit memory: Effects of elaboration depend on unitization.American Journal of Psychology, 102, 151–181.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shimamura, A. P. (1986). Priming effects in amnesia: Evidence for a dissociable memory function.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38A, 619–644.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sidis, B. (1898).The psychology of suggestion. New York: Appleton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weldon, M. S. (1991). Mechanisms underlying priming on perceptual tests.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 526–541.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weldon, M. S., Roediger, H. L., & Challis, B. F. (1989). The properties of retrieval cues constrain the picture superiority effect.Memory & Cognition, 17, 95–105.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Brian D. Mackenzie.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Langsford, P.B., Mackenzie, B.D. Unconscious influences on direct-test performance. Psychol. Res 58, 134–143 (1995). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00571101

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00571101

Keywords

Navigation