Advertisement

Reading and Writing

, Volume 8, Issue 2, pp 171–188 | Cite as

The early manifestation of developmental dyslexia: Evidence from German children

  • Heinz Wimmer
Article

Abstract

The present study retrospectively examined early difficulties with phonological coding and phonemic segmentation of German children who after four years in school were diagnosed as dyslexic. German, in comparison to English, exhibits rather simple and straight-forward grapheme-phoneme correspondences, and the initial teaching approach was phonics oriented. Despite these favorable circumstances for the acquisition of phonological coding, the majority of the later dyslexic children had particular difficulties with the accurate reading of nonwords and of unfamiliar words after about seven months of reading instruction. However, there were enormous differences between the dyslexic children. Two of them were completely unable to ‘blend’ phonemes into pronunciations, another seven were slow and error prone decoders, and three children had slow and laborious pronunciation assembly as the core problem. The majority of the later dyslexic children also exhibited phonemic segmentation deficits as tested with a nonword spelling task and a phoneme reversal task. In correspondence with findings from older German dyslexic children, the early difficulties with accurate phonological coding and phonemic segmentation were no longer found at the end of grade four. Children then suffered from very slow reading and poor spelling. In general, the difficulties of German dyslexic children emphasize the phonological impairment account of dyslexia. More specifically, these findings suggest that the assembly of letter sounds into pronunciations is particularly affected in the early phase of learning to read a consistent orthography.

Key words

Dyslexia Early reading and spelling difficulties German orthography Nonword reading Phonological deficits 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Bruck, M. (1990). Word recognition skills of adults with childhood diagnosis of dyslexia,Developmental Psychology 26: 439–454.Google Scholar
  2. Cossu, G., Shankweiler, D., Liberman, I., Katz, L. & Tola, G. (1988). Awareness of phonological segments and reading ability in Italian children,Applied Psycholinguistics 2: 1–16.Google Scholar
  3. Cossu, G., Shankweiler, D., Liberman, I. Y. & Gugliotta, M. (1995). Visual and phonological determinants of misreadings in a transparent orthography,Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 7: 237–256.Google Scholar
  4. Frith, U. (1985). Beneath the surface of development dyslexia. In: K. E. Patterson, J. C. Marshall & M. Coltheart (eds.),Surface dyslexia (pp. 303–332). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  5. Levelt, W. J. M. (1989).Speaking: From intention to articulation. Cambridge: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
  6. Lovett, M. W. (1987). A developmental approach to reading disability: Accuracy and speed criteria of normal and deficient reading skill,Child Development 58: 234–260.Google Scholar
  7. Mann, V. A. (1984). Longitudinal prediction and prevention of early reading difficulty,Annals of Dyslexia 34: 117–136.Google Scholar
  8. Ognjenovic, V., Lukatela, G., Feldman, L. & Turvey, M. T. (1983). Misreading by beginning readers of Serbo-Croatian,Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 35: 581–615.Google Scholar
  9. Oney, B. & Goldman, S. R. (1984). Decoding and comprehension skills in Turkish and English: Effects of the regularity of grapheme-phoneme correspondences,Journal of Educational Psychology 76: 557–568.Google Scholar
  10. Porpodas, C. D., Pantelis, S. N. & Hantziou, E. (1990). Phonological and lexical encoding processes in beginning readers: Effects of age and word characteristics,Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 2: 197–208.Google Scholar
  11. Rack, J. P., Snowling, M. J. & Olson, R. (1992). The nonword reading deficit in developmental dyslexia: A review,Reading Research Quarterly 27: 29–53.Google Scholar
  12. Sprenger-Charolles, L. & Casalis, S. (1995). Reading and spelling acquisition in French first graders: Longitudinal evidence,Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 7: 39–63.Google Scholar
  13. Stanovich, K. E. (1994). Annotation: Does dyslexia exist?,Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 35: 579–595.Google Scholar
  14. Thorstad, G. (1991). The effect of orthography on the acquisition of literacy skills,British Journal of Psychology 82: 527–537.Google Scholar
  15. Venetzky, R. L. (1970).The structure of English orthography. The Hague/Paris: Mouton.Google Scholar
  16. Wimmer, H. (1995). The nonword reading deficit in developmental dyslexia: Evidence from German children,Journal of Experimental Child Psychology.Google Scholar
  17. Wimmer, H. (1993). Characteristics of developmental dyslexia in a regular writing system,Applied Psycholinguistics 14: 1–33.Google Scholar
  18. Wimmer, H. & Frith, U. (1993). Orthographies and learning to read: An English-German comparison. Unpublished manuscript. University of Salzburg, Austria.Google Scholar
  19. Wimmer, H. & Goswami, U. (1994). The influence of orthographic consistency on reading development: Word recognition in English and German children,Cognition 51: 91–103.Google Scholar
  20. Wimmer, H. & Hummer, P. (1990). How German speaking first graders read and spell: Doubts on the importance of the logographic stage,Applied Psycholinguistics 11: 349–368.Google Scholar
  21. Wimmer, H., Landerl, K., Linortner, R. & Hummer, P. (1991). The relationship of phonemic awareness to reading acquisition: More consequence than precondition but still important,Cognition 40: 219–249.Google Scholar
  22. Wolf, M. (1991). Naming and reading: The contribution of the cognitive neurosciences,Reading Research Quarterly 26: 123–140.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Kluwer Academic Publishers 1996

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.University of SalzburgAustria

Personalised recommendations