Skip to main content
Log in

Personality components of decision making

II. Relationships between decision-making behavior and behavioral stereotypy proneness

  • Published:
Psychologische Forschung Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Summary

Relationships between proneness to behavioral stereotypy (Ster) measured in a problem-solving task (electromaze) and decision-making behavior (under conditions of risk and certainty, chance and skill) were investigated using 64 subjects. Subjects more stereotyped in the electromaze (Ster +) tended to also be more stereotyped in their decision-making behavior. Ster was not related to extraversion, neuroticism, rigidity or overall electromaze performance, but Ster more often required an excessive number of trials for task solution. The decision making of males tended to be more stereotyped. The results were discussed in terms of the construct validity and theoretical interpretation of Ster.

Zusammenfassung

Es wurden Beziehungen zwischen der Neigung zur Verhaltensstereotypie (Ster), die bei einer Problemlösungsaufgabe gemessen wurde, und dem Entscheidungsverhalten (mit und ohne Risiko, wobei der Erfolg sowohl durch den Zufall wie auch durch das Können der Vp bestimmt wurde) untersucht. Vpn, die sich im Electromaze stereotyp verhielten (Ster +), verhielten sich auch bei ihren Entscheidungen stereotyp. Es wurden keine Beziehungen zwischen Ster und den Variablen Extraversion, Neurotizismus, Rigidität und Gesamtleistung im Electromaze gefunden, jedoch benötigten Ster häufiger eine extrem lange Serie von Versuchen zur Aufgabenlösung. Die männliche Vpn neigten stärker dazu, in Entscheidungssituationen stereotyp zu reagieren. Die Ergebnisse wurden vom Standpunkt der Konstruktvalidität und der theoretischen Grundlage der Ster aus diskutiert.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Aborn, M., Rubenstein, H.: Information theory and immediate recall. J. exp. Psychol. 44, 260–266 (1952).

    Google Scholar 

  • Alexander, F., French, T. M.: Psychoanalytic therapy. New York: Ronald 1946.

    Google Scholar 

  • Armitage, S. G., Brown, C. R., Denny, M. R.: Stereotypy of responses in schizophrenics. J. clin. Psychol. 20, 225–230 (1964).

    Google Scholar 

  • Atkinson, J. W., Bastian, J. R., Earl, R. W., Litwin, G. H.: The achievement motive, goal setting, and probability preferences. J. abnorm. soc. Psychol. 60, 27–36 (1960).

    Google Scholar 

  • Attneave, F.: Applications of information theory to psychology. New York: Holt 1959.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berlyne, D. E.: Uncertainty and conflict: a point of contact between informationtheory and behavior-theory concepts. Psychol. Rev. 64, 329–339 (1957).

    Google Scholar 

  • —: Conflict, arousal and curiosity. New York: McGraw-Hill 1960.

    Google Scholar 

  • —: Motivational problems raised by exploratory and epistemic behavior. In: Koch, S. (Ed.), Psychology: a study of a science, Vol. 5, 284–364 New York: McGraw-Hill 1963.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. S., Goodnow, J. J., Austin, G. A.: A study of thinking. New York: Wiley 1956.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brengelmann, J. C., Brengelmann, L.: Deutsche Validierung von Fragebogen der Extraversion, neurotischen Tendenz und Rigidität. Z. exp. angew. Psychol. 7, 291–331 (1960a).

    Google Scholar 

  • — —: Deutsche Validierung von Fragebogen dogmatischer und intoleranter Haltungen. Z. exp. angew. Psychol. 7, 451–471 (1960b).

    Google Scholar 

  • Carron, A. V.: Intra-task reliability and specificity of individual consistency. Perceptual and Motor Skills 30, 583–587 (1970).

    Google Scholar 

  • Cartwright, D., Festinger, L.: A quantitative theory of decision. Psychol. Rev. 50, 595–621 (1943).

    Google Scholar 

  • Chown, S. M.: Rigidity — a flexible concept. Psychol. Bull. 56, 195–223 (1959).

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J., Hansel, C. E. M.: Risk and gambling. London: Philosophical Library 1956.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cronbach, L. J., Meehl, P. E.: Construct validity in psychological tests. Psychol. Bull. 52, 281–302 (1955).

    Google Scholar 

  • Cowden, J. E.: Prediction enhancement through the use of moderator variables. J. cons. clin. Psychol. 33, 621–624 (1969).

    Google Scholar 

  • Davis, G. A.: Current status of research and theory in human problem solving. Psychol. Bull. 66, 36–54 (1966).

    Google Scholar 

  • de Charms, R., Prafulachandra, N. D.: Hope of success, fear of failure, subjective probability, and risk-taking behavior. J. pers. soc. Psychol. 1, 558–568 (1965).

    Google Scholar 

  • Dember, W. N.: Alternation behavior. In: Fiske, D. W., Maddi, S. R. (Eds.), Functions of varied experience. Homeward (Ill.) Dorsey 1961.

    Google Scholar 

  • —, Fowler, H.: Spontaneous alternation behavior. Psychol. Bull. 55, 412–428 (1958).

    Google Scholar 

  • Drever, J.: A dictionary of psychology. Harmondsworth, England: Penguin 1964.

    Google Scholar 

  • Edwards, W.: Probability preferences in gambling. Amer. J. Psychol. 66, 349–364 (1953).

    Google Scholar 

  • —: Methods for computing uncertainties. Amer. J. Psychol. 67, 164–170 (1954a).

    Google Scholar 

  • Edwards, W.: Probability preferences among bets with differing expected values. Amer. J. Psychol. 67, 56–67 (1954b).

    Google Scholar 

  • —: The reliability of probability preferences. Amer. J. Psychol. 67, 68–95 (1954c).

    Google Scholar 

  • —: The theory of decision making. Psychol. Bull. 51, 380–417 (1954d).

    Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, H. J.: Dimensions of personality. London: Routledge, Kegan Paul, 1947.

    Google Scholar 

  • —: Cortical inhibition, figural after effect, and theory of personality. J. abnorm. soc. Psychol. 51, 94–106 (1955).

    Google Scholar 

  • —: The structure of human personality, Third Ed. London: Methuen 1970.

    Google Scholar 

  • —, Levey, A.: Alternation in choice behavior and extraversion. Life Sci. 4, 115–119 (1965).

    Google Scholar 

  • Fattu, N. A., Mech, E., Kapos, E.: Some statistical relationships between selected response dimensions and problem-solving efficiency. Psychol. Monogr. 68, No. 6 (1954).

    Google Scholar 

  • —, Kapos, E., Mech, E. V.: Response “predictability” in a complex multiple choice situation. J. gen. Psychol. 54, 263–270 (1956).

    Google Scholar 

  • Festinger, L.: Studies in decision: I. Decision-time, relative frequency of judgment and subjective confidence as related to physical stimulus differences. J. exp. Psychol. 32, 291–301 (1943).

    Google Scholar 

  • Fiske, D. W., Maddi, S. R. (Eds.): Functions of varied experience. Homeward (Ill.) Dorsey 1961.

    Google Scholar 

  • —, Rice, L.: Intra-individual variability. Psychol. Bull. 52, 217–250 (1955).

    Google Scholar 

  • Ghiselli, E. E.: The prediction of predictability. Educ. Psychol. Measurement 20, 3–8 (1960).

    Google Scholar 

  • Glanzer, M.: The role of stimulus satiation in spontaneous alternation. J. exp. Psychol. 45, 387–393 (1953a).

    Google Scholar 

  • —: Stimulus satiation: An explanation of spontaneous alternation and related phenomena. Psychol. Rev. 560, 257–268 (1953b).

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldsmith, R. W.: Persönlichkeitsspezifische Komponenten des Entscheidungsverhaltens. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, Freie Universität Berlin 1967.

  • —: Persönlichkeitsspezifische Komponenten des Entscheidungsverhaltens. I. Beziehungen zwischen der Interferenzneigung und dem Entscheidungsverhalten. (Personality components of decision making. I. Relationships between decisionmaking behavior and interference proneness.) Psychol. Forsch. 32, 135–168 (1968).

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldstone, S.: Variability of temporal judgment: intersensory comparisons and sex differences. Perceptual and Motor Skills 26, 211–215 (1968).

    Google Scholar 

  • Guilford, J. P.: The nature of human intelligence. New York: McGraw-Hill 1967.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hackl, H.: Untersuchungen zur Variabilität des Verhaltens bei Normalen und Schizophrenen. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, University of Vienna 1967.

  • Hebb, D. O.: The organization of behavior. New York: Wiley 1949.

    Google Scholar 

  • —: Drives and the C.N.S. (conceptual nervous system). Psychol. Rev. 62, 243–254 (1955).

    Google Scholar 

  • Holman, E. W.: Tests for spontaneous alternation. Psychol. Rev. 72, 427–436 (1966).

    Google Scholar 

  • Hörmann, H.: Konflikt und Entscheidung. Göttingen: Hogrefe 1960.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L.: Principles of behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts 1943.

    Google Scholar 

  • Irwin, F. W., Smith, W. A. S., Mayfield, J. F.: Tests of two theories of decision in an “expanded judgment” situation. J. exp. Psychol. 51, 261–268 (1956).

    Google Scholar 

  • — —: Further tests of theories of decision in an “expanded judgment” situation. J. exp. Psychol. 52, 345–348 (1956).

    Google Scholar 

  • — —: Value, cost, and information as determiners of decision. J. exp. Psychol. 54, 229–232 (1957).

    Google Scholar 

  • Klix, F.: Psychophysik kognitiver Prozesse. In: Marko, H., Färber, G. (Eds.), Kybernetik 1968 (Report of Kongreß der Deutschen Gesellschaft für Kybernetik, München 1968). München: Oldenbourg 1968.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kogan, N., Wallach, M. A.: Risk taking: A study in cognition and personality. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston 1964.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kubie, L. S.: The repetitive core of neurosis. Psychoanal. Quart. 10, 23–43 (1941).

    Google Scholar 

  • —: The fundamental nature of the distinction between normality and neurosis. Psychoanal. Quart. 23, 167–204 (1954).

    Google Scholar 

  • Lippmann, W.: Public opinion. New York: Harcourt, Brace, 1922.

    Google Scholar 

  • Liverant, S., Scodel, A.: Internal and external control as determinants of decision making under conditions of risk. Psychol. Rep. 7, 59–67 (1960).

    Google Scholar 

  • Maddi, S. R.: Personality theories. A comparative analysis. Homewood (Ill.): Dorsey 1968.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. A.: Free recall of redundant strings of digits. J. exp. Psychol. 56, 485–491 (1958).

    Google Scholar 

  • —, Frick, F. C.: Statistical behavioristics and sequences of responses. Psychol. Rev. 56, 311–324 (1949).

    Google Scholar 

  • Mittenecker, E.: Die Analyse „zufälliger“ Reaktionsfolgen. Z. exp. angew. Psychol. 5, 45–60 (1958).

    Google Scholar 

  • Montgomery, K. C.: Exploratory behavior and its relation to spontaneous alternation in a series of maze exposures. J. comp. physiol. Psychol. 45, 50–57 (1952a).

    Google Scholar 

  • —: A test of two explanations of spontaneous alternation. J. comp. physiol. Psychol. 45, 287–295 (1952b).

    Google Scholar 

  • Newman, E. B.: Computational methods useful in analyzing series of binary data. Amer. J. Psychol. 64, 252–262 (1951).

    Google Scholar 

  • Ramsey, R. W., Broadhurst, A.: The non-randomness of attempts at random responses: Relationships with personality variables and psychiatric disorder. Brit. J. Psychol. 59, 299–304 (1968).

    Google Scholar 

  • Rupprecht, A.: Variabilität des Verhaltens und Intelligenz bei Normalen und Neurotikern. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, University of Vienna 1957.

  • Saunders, D. R.: Moderator variables in prediction. Educ. Psychol. Measurement 16, 209–222 (1956).

    Google Scholar 

  • Schellenberg, J. A.: An introduction to social psychology. New York: Random House 1970.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schultz, D. P.: Spontaneous alternation behavior in humans. Psychol. Bull. 62, 394–400 (1964).

    Google Scholar 

  • Shannon, C. E.: A mathematical theory of communication. Bell Systems Technical Journal 27, 379–423, 623–656 (1948). Reprinted in Shannon, C. E., Weaver, W.: The mathematical theory of communication. Urban: The University of Illinois Press 19549.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, S.: Theoretical models of choice and strategy behavior: Stable state behavior in the two-choice uncertain outcome situation. Psychometrika 24, 303–316 (1959).

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. A.: Theories of decision-making in economics and behavioral science. Amer. Economic Rev. 3, 253–283 (1959).

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, G. J. W., Klein, G. S.: Cognitive controls in serial behavior patterns. J. Personality 22, 188–213 (1953).

    Google Scholar 

  • Stroop, J. R.: Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J. exp. Psychol. 18, 643–661 (1935).

    Google Scholar 

  • Tune, G. S.: Response preferences: a review of some relevant literature. Psychol. Bull. 61, 286–302 (1964).

    Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A.: Intransitivity of preferences. Psychol. Rev. 76, 31–48 (1969).

    Google Scholar 

  • Walker, E. L.: The duration and course of the reaction decrement and the influence of reward. J. comp. physiol. Psychol. 49, 167–176 (1956).

    Google Scholar 

  • —, Dember, W. N., Earl, R. W., Karoly, A. J.: Choice alternation: I. Stimulus vs. place vs. response. J. comp. physiol. Psychol. 48, 19–23 (1955a).

    Google Scholar 

  • — — —, Fliege, S. W., Karoly, A. J.: Choice alternation: II. Exposure to stimulus und place without choice. J. comp. physiol. Psychol. 48, 24–28 (1955b).

    Google Scholar 

  • — — —, Fawl, C. L., Karoly, A. J., Choice alternation: III. Response intensity vs. response discriminability. J. comp. physiol. Psychol. 48, 1955, 80–85 (1955c).

    Google Scholar 

  • Weiss, R. L.: On producing random responses. Psychol. Rep. 14, 931–941 (1964).

    Google Scholar 

  • Worell, L.: Intraindividual instability and conflict. J. abnorm. soc. Psychol. 66, 480–488 (1963).

    Google Scholar 

  • Zeaman, D., House, B. J.: The growth and decay of reactive inhibition as measured by alternation behavior. J. exp. Psychol. 41, 177–186 (1951).

    Google Scholar 

  • Zeblewski, E.: Die Anwendbarkeit des Perseverationstests von Mittenecker für die Persönlichkeitsdiagnostik. Z. exp. angew. Psychol. 6, 310–320 (1959).

    Google Scholar 

  • Zlotowski, M.: Behavioral differences between process and reactive schizophrenics in a monotonous repetitive task. J. pers. soc. Psychol. 1, 240–244 (1965).

    Google Scholar 

  • —, Bakan, P.: Behavioral variability of process and reactive schizophrenics in a binary guessing task. J. abnorm. soc. Psychol. 66, 185–187 (1963).

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Goldsmith, R.W. Personality components of decision making. Psychol. Forsch. 35, 187–212 (1972). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00542690

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00542690

Keywords

Navigation