Advertisement

Self-selected exercise intensity is unchanged by sleep loss

  • Bruce J. Martin
  • Ruth Haney
Article

Summary

Sleep loss alters the perceptual response to exercise: subjects describe constant external work loads as more severe after sleep deprivation. However, since subjects cannot be “blind” to their sleep status and have knowledge of previous exertional ratings, it remains unknown if this increase in perceived exertion merely represents expectations of increased difficulty of exercise after sleep loss. As one approach to this problem, we asked 24 subjects to produce equivalent “very hard” efforts, once after normal sleep, and once after 30 h without sleep. This was done by allowing the subject, while walking at constant speed, to adjust treadmill grade, without knowledge of the actual elevation. We found that exercise at equal perceived exertion was associated with the choice of a nearly equal absolute work load after sleep deprivation as after normal sleep (17.1 vs. 17.5% grade; p=n.s.). In addition, after 10 min of exercise at the self-selected intensity, subjects displayed identical ventilation, oxygen uptake, and CO2 production. However, heart rate was significantly lower during exercise after sleep loss (170±3 vs. 178±3 b·min−1; p<0.001). These results suggest that previously measured increases in perceived exertion during constant-load exercise after sleep loss may be spurious.

Key words

Oxygen uptake Ventilation Heart rate Perceived exertion 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Borg G (1974) Perceived exertion. In: Wilmore J (ed) Exercise and sports science reviews. Academic Press, New York, pp 131–153Google Scholar
  2. Bonnet MH (1980) Sleep, performance, and mood after energy expenditure equivalent of 40 hours of sleep deprivation. Psychophysiology 17: 56–63Google Scholar
  3. Brodan V, Vostechovsky M, Kuhn M, Cepelak J (1969) Changes of mental and physical performance of sleep deprived healthy volunteers. Activ Nerv Super 11: 175–181Google Scholar
  4. Copes K, Rosentszweig J (1972) The effects of sleep deprivation upon motor performance of ninth-grade students. J Sports Med Phys Fitness 12: 47–53Google Scholar
  5. Edwards RHT, Melcher A, Hesser C, Wigertz O, Eklund LG (1972) Physiological correlates of perceived exertion in continuous and intermittent exercise with the same average power output. Eur J Clin Invest 2: 108–114Google Scholar
  6. Ekblom B, Goldbarg A (1971) The influence of physical training and other factors on the subjective rating of perceived exertion. Acta Physiol Scand 83: 399–406Google Scholar
  7. Evans WO (1963) A titration schedule on a treadmill. J Exp Anal Behav 6: 219–221Google Scholar
  8. Gerber M, House J (1969) Hypoxia and self-paced work. Percept Mot Skills 28: 995–1002Google Scholar
  9. Gerber M, House J, Winsmann F (1972) Self-paced ergometer performance: effects of pedal resistance, motivational contingency, and inspired oxygen concentration. Percept Mot Skills 34: 875–881Google Scholar
  10. Gibra I (1973) Probability and statistical inference for scientists and engineers. Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, pp 283–302Google Scholar
  11. Hoddes E, Zarcone V, Smythe H, Phillips R, Dement W (1975) Quantification of sleepiness: a new approach. Psychophysiology 10: 431–436Google Scholar
  12. Holland G (1968) Effects of limited sleep deprivation on performance of selected motor tasks. Res Q 39: 285–294Google Scholar
  13. Holmgren G, Harker G (1967) Characteristic pace as determined by the use of a tracking treadmill. J Appl Psychol 51: 278–283Google Scholar
  14. Hord D, Tracy M, Lubin A, Johnson L (1975) Effect of self-enhanced EEG alpha on performance and mood after two nights of sleep loss. Psychophysiology 12: 585–590Google Scholar
  15. Horne JA (1978) A review of the biological effects of total sleep deprivation in man. Biol Psychol 7: 55–102Google Scholar
  16. Horstman D, Weiskopf R, Robinson S (1979) The nature of the perception of effort at sea level and high altitude. Med Sci Sports 11: 150–154Google Scholar
  17. Hughes A, Goldman R (1970) Energy cost of “hard work.” J Appl Physiol 29: 570–572Google Scholar
  18. Koller E, Starter G, Palmer J, Doctor R, Mandell A (1966) Stress in subjects undergoing sleep deprivation. Psychosom Med 28: 101–113Google Scholar
  19. Lollgen H, Ulmer H, Nieding G (1977) Heart rate and perceptual response to exercise with different pedalling speed in normal subjects and patients. Eur J Appl Physiol 37: 297–304Google Scholar
  20. Martin B (1981) Effect of sleep deprivation on tolerance of prolonged exercise. Eur J Appl Physiol 47: 345–354Google Scholar
  21. Morgan W (1973) Psychological factors influencing perceived exertion. Med Sci Sports 5: 97–103Google Scholar
  22. McNair D, Lorr M, Droppleman L (1971) Manual for the POMS. Education and Industrial Testing Service, San DiegoGoogle Scholar
  23. Opstad P, Ekanger R, Nummestad M, Raabe N (1978) Performance, mood, and clinical symptoms in men exposed to prolonged heavy physical exercise and sleep deprivation. Aviat Space Environ Med 49: 1065–1073Google Scholar
  24. Patton J, Morgan W, Vogel J (1977) Perceived exertion of absolute work during a military physical training program. Eur J Appl Physiol 36: 107–114Google Scholar
  25. Peeke S, Callaway E, Jones R, Stone G, Doyle J (1980) Combined effects of alcohol and sleep deprivation in normal young adults. Psychopharmacology 67: 279–287Google Scholar
  26. Pickett G, Morris A (1975) Effects of acute sleep and food deprivation on total body response time and cardiovascular performance. J Sports Med 15: 49–56Google Scholar
  27. Soule R, Goldman R (1973) Pacing of intermittent work during 31 hours. Med Sci Sports 5: 128–131Google Scholar
  28. Webb WB, Agnew HW Jr (1973) Effects on performance of high and low energy-expenditure during sleep deprivation. Percept Mot Skills 37: 511–514Google Scholar
  29. Zuckerman M, Lubin B (1965) Manual for the Mutiple Affect Adjective Check List. Educational and Industrial Testing Service, San DiegoGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 1982

Authors and Affiliations

  • Bruce J. Martin
    • 1
  • Ruth Haney
    • 1
  1. 1.Physiology Section, Medical Sciences Program and Department of PsychologyIndiana UniversityBloomingtonUSA

Personalised recommendations