Advertisement

Reading and Writing

, Volume 8, Issue 1, pp 61–72 | Cite as

An exploratory study of morphological errors in children's written stories

Article

Abstract

This exploratory study provides descriptive information about the use of morphologically complex words in the story writing of second and third graders, some with and some without learning disabilities (LD). The purpose was to determine how commonly and how accurately children used inflections, derivations, and compound words spontaneously in their writing. The results showed significant differences by grade level and group (LD and Non-LD) in the frequency with which morphologically complex words are used. The LD second graders were less accurate than their peers in their use of morphological markers; both LD and Non-LD third graders had high levels of accuracy. In addition, examination of morphological markers that children use in written but nor oral language showed growth from second to third grade. The results suggest that the second and third grades may be a transitional period, in which the children are consolidating their knowledge of inflected forms and just beginning to use derived forms in their spontaneous writing. Reasons that children might make morphological errors in writing and problems that might be addressed in future studies are discussed.

Key words

Learning disabilities Morphological errors Second and third graders Story writing 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Beers, C.S. & Beers, J.W. (1992). Children's spelling of English inflectional morphology. In: S. Templeton & D.R. Bear (eds.), Development of orthographic knowledge and the foundations of literacy (pp. 231–251). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  2. Beery, K.E. (1982). Revised administration, scoring, and teaching manual for the developmental test of visual-motor integration. Cleveland, OH: Curriculum Press.Google Scholar
  3. Brown, R. (1973). A first language: The early stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  4. Berko, J. (1958). The child's learning of English morphology, Word 14: 150–177.Google Scholar
  5. Carlisle, J.F. (1988). Knowledge of derivational morphology and spelling ability in fourth, sixth, and eighth graders, Applied Psycholinguistics 9: 247–266.Google Scholar
  6. Carlisle, J.F. (1994). Morphological awareness, spelling, and story writing: Possible relationships for elementary-age learning-disabled and non-learning-disabled children. In: N. Jordan & J. Goldsmith-Phillips (eds.), Learning disabilities: New directions for assessment and intervention (pp. 123–145). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.Google Scholar
  7. Champion, A. (1992). Knowledge of suffixed words in reading and oral language contexts: A comparison of reading disabled and normal readers. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Northwestern University.Google Scholar
  8. Dunn, L.M. & Dunn, L.M. (1981). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
  9. Houck, C.K. & Billingsley, B.C. (1989). Written expression of students with and without learning disabilities: Differences across the grades, Journal of Learning Disabilities 22: 561–567, 572.Google Scholar
  10. Jastak, S. & Wilkinson, G.S. (1984). Wide Range Achievement Test-Revised. Wilmington, DE: Jastak Associates.Google Scholar
  11. Johnson, D.J. & Grant, J.O. (1989). Written narratives of normal and learning disabled students, Annals of Dyslexia 39: 140–158.Google Scholar
  12. Juel, C. (1988). Learning to read and write: A longitudinal study of 54 children from first through fourth grades, Journal of Educational Psychology 80: 437–447.Google Scholar
  13. Lynch, E.M. & Jones, S.D. (1989). Process and product: A review of the research on LD children's writing skills, Learning Disability Quarterly 12: 74–86.Google Scholar
  14. Myklebust, H.R. (1965). Development and disorders of written language, Vol. 1. New York: Grune & Stratton.Google Scholar
  15. Nodine, B.F., Barenbaum, E. & Newcomer, P. (1985). Story composition by learning disabled, reading disabled, and normal children, Learning Disability Quarterly 8: 167–179.Google Scholar
  16. Rubin, H. (1988). Morphological knowledge and early writing ability, Language and Speech 31: 337–355.Google Scholar
  17. Rubin, H., Patterson, P.A. & Kantor, M. (1991). Morphological development and writing ability in children and adults, Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools 22: 228–235.Google Scholar
  18. Scardamalia, M. (1981). How children cope with the cognitive demands of writing. In: C.H. Frederiksen & J.F. Dominic (eds.), Writing: The nature, development, and teaching of written communication, Vol 2 (pp. 81–103). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  19. Tyler, A. & Nagy, W. (1989). The acquisition of English derivational morphology Journal of Memory and Language 28: 649–667.Google Scholar
  20. Vogel, S.A. (1983). A qualitative analysis of morphological ability in learning disabled and achieving children, Journal of Learning Disabilities 16: 416–420.Google Scholar
  21. Wiig, E.H., Semel, E.M. & Crouse, M.A.B. (1973). The use of English morphology by high-risk and learning disabled children, Journal of Learning Disabilities 6: 59–67.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Kluwer Academic Publishers 1996

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Northwestern UniversityEvanstonUSA

Personalised recommendations