Skip to main content
Log in

Positive and negative instructions in symbolic paired comparisons with the months of the year

  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Summary

We investigated the semantic interpretation of positive (MORE THAN) and negative (LESS THAN) instructions in a symbolic paired comparison task with an overlearned and finite series of stimuli. French-speaking subjects were asked to indicate which member of month pairs occurred PLUS TOT (earlier), PLUS TARD (later), MOINS TOT (less early), or MOINS TARD (less late) in the year. In Experiment 1, the sign of the comparative instruction (MORE vs. LESS) was kept constant, while its attribute (EARLY vs. LATE) varied. In Experiment 2, the attribute of the instruction was kept constant, while its sign varied. The main results indicate that (1) negative instructions (with adverb LESS) yielded longer response latencies than positive ones (with adverb MORE); this difference was particularly salient on the first experimental session but, on subsequent sessions, it was reduced in Experiment 1 and neutralized in Experiment 2; (2) a symbolic-distance effect was observed on response latency and response accuracy with the four forms of the comparative instruction; (3) violations of monotonicity were noted in the symbolic-distance effect when June was used as a reference month in the analysis; (4) the serial-position effect had the form of a double-bowed curve with three stable anchor points around January, June, and December; and (5) the pattern of semantic-congruity effect with negative instructions was the exact reversal of that obtained with positive instructions; that is, if the month pairs occurred early in the year, response latencies were shorter with the instruction “less late” than with “less early,” whereas if the month pairs occurred late in the year response latencies were shorter with instruction "less early’ than with “less late.” The theoretical implications of these results for Banks' Semantic-coding model and Holyoak's Reference-point model are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Banks, W. P. (1977). Encoding and processing of symbolic information in comparative judgments. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 11). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Banks, W. P., Clark, H. H., & Lucy, P. (1975). The locus of the semantic congruity effect in comparative judgments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 104, 35–47.

    Google Scholar 

  • Banks, W. P., & Flora, J. (1977). Semantic and perceptual processes in symbolic comparisons. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 3, 278–290.

    Google Scholar 

  • Banks, W. P., Fujii, M., & Kayra-Stuart, F. (1976). Semantic congruity effects in comparative judgments of magnitudes of digits. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2, 435–447.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chase, W. G., & Ericsson, K. A. (1982). Skill and working memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), Advances in learning and motivation (Vol. 16). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, H. H. (1969). Linguistic processes in deductive reasoning. Psychological Review, 76, 387–404.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, H. H., & Clark, E. V. (1977). Psychology and language. New York: Harcourt, Brace, Jovanovich, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corcia, M. (1989). Facteurs de complexité sémantique dans la compréhension des énoncés comparatifs en anglais chez L'enfant [Factors of semantic complexity in the comprehension of English comparative statements by children]. Bulletin de Psychologie, XLI (383), 291–301.

    Google Scholar 

  • Desrochers, A., & Petrusic, W. M. (1983). Comprehension effects in comparative judgments. In J. C. Yuille (Ed.), Imagery, memory and cognition: Essays in honour of Allan Paivio. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fairbanks, B. A. (1969). Experiments on the temporal aspects of number perception (Doctoral dissertation, University of Arizona, 1969). Dissertation Abstracts International, 30, 403 b (University Microfilms No. 60-10,052).

  • Flores D'Arcais, G. B. (1974). Semantic and perceptual factors in the processing of comparative sentences. Italian Journal of Psychology, 1, 267–303.

    Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, W. J. (1983). Image and verbal processes in reasoning about the months of the year. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 9, 650–666.

    Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, W. J. (1984). Analog and semantic models of judgments about the months of the year. Memory & Cognition, 12, 306–313.

    Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, W. J. (1990). About time: Inventing the fourth dimension. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grenzebach, A. P., & McDonald, J. E. (1992). Alphabetic sequence decisions for letter pairs with separations of one to three letters. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 865–872.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holyoak, K. (1978). Comparative judgments with numerical reference points. Cognitive Psychology, 10, 203–243.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holyoak, K. J., & Patterson, K. K. (1981). A positional discriminability model of linear-order judgments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7, 1283–1302.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jamieson, D. G., & Petrusic, W. M. (1975). Relational judgments with remembered stimuli. Perception & Psychophysics, 18, 373–378.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klahr, D., Chase, W. G., & Lovelace, E. A. (1983). Structure and process in alphabetic retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 9, 462–477.

    Google Scholar 

  • Le Ny, J.-F. (1979). La sémantique psychologique [Psychological semantics]. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marks, D. F. (1972). Relative judgment: A phenomenon and a theory. Perception & Psychophysics, 11, 156–160.

    Google Scholar 

  • McNamara, T. P., & Miller, D. L. (1989). Attributes of theories of meaning. Psychological Bulletin, 106, 355–376.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moyer, R. S., & Dumais, S. T. (1978). Mental comparison. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 12). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Petrusic, W. M., & Baranski, J. V. (1989). Semantic congruity effects in perceptual comparisons. Perception & Psychophysics, 45, 439–452.

    Google Scholar 

  • Poltrock, S. E. (1989). A random walk model of digit comparison. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 33, 131–162.

    Google Scholar 

  • Segui, J., & Bertoncini, J. (1978). La vérification des phrases comparatives [Verification of comparative sentences]. L'Année Psychologique, 78, 129–144.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seymour, P. H. K. (1980a). Semantic and structural coding of the months. British Journal of Psychology, 71, 379–393.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seymour, P. H. K. (1980b). Internal representation of the months. Psychological Research, 42, 255–273.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woocher, F. D., Glass, A. L., & Holyoak, K. J. (1978). Positional discriminability in linear orderings. Memory & Cognition, 6, 165–173.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Gélinas, C.S., Desrochers, A. Positive and negative instructions in symbolic paired comparisons with the months of the year. Psychol. Res 55, 40–51 (1993). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00419892

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00419892

Keywords

Navigation