Advertisement

Psychological Research

, Volume 55, Issue 1, pp 15–19 | Cite as

Aging and temporal discrimination of brief auditory intervals

  • Thomas H. Rammsayer
  • Susan D. Lima
  • Wolfgang H. Vogel
Article

Summary

In a duration-discrimination experiment, young adults (mean age = 25.1), middle-aged adults (mean age = 45.5), and older adults (mean age = 64.6) were presented with two very brief auditorily marked intervals per trial, and their task was to decide which of the two was longer in duration. An adaptive psychophysical procedure was used to determine difference thresholds in relation to a constant standard interval of 50 ms. It was found that duration-discrimination performance was unaffected by age; all three age groups yielded a difference threshold of approximately 17 ms. It was concluded that the ability to discriminate durations of very brief auditory intervals appears to be based on an underlying timing mechanism that does not slow down with advancing adult age.

Keywords

Young Adult Difference Threshold Marked Interval Temporal Discrimination Timing Mechanism 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Block, R. A. (1990). Models of psychological time. In R. A. Block (Ed.), Cognitive models of psychological time (pp. 1–35). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  2. Church, R. M. (1984). Properties of the internal clock. In J. Gibbon & L. G. Allan (Eds.), Timing and time perception (pp. 566–582). New York: New York Academy of Sciences.Google Scholar
  3. Cranford, J. L., & Stream, R. W. (1991). Discrimination of short duration tones by elderly subjects. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 46, P37–41.Google Scholar
  4. Creelman, C. D. (1962). Human discrimination of auditory duration. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 34, 582–593.Google Scholar
  5. Cronbach, L. J. (1990). Essentials of psychological testing. New York: Harper and Row.Google Scholar
  6. Feifel, H. (1957). Judgment of time in younger and older persons. Journal of Gerontology, 12, 71–74.Google Scholar
  7. Florentine, M., & Buns, S. (1984). Temporal gap detection in sensorineural and simulated hearing impairments. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 27, 449–455.Google Scholar
  8. Gengel, R. W. (1973). Temporal effects in frequency discrimination by hearing-impaired listeners. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 54, 11–15.Google Scholar
  9. Goldstone, S., Boardman, W. K., & Lhamon, W. T. (1958). Kinesthetic clues in the development of time concepts. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 93, 185–190.Google Scholar
  10. Ison, J. R., O'Connor, K., Bowen, G. P., & Bocimea, A. (1991). Temporal resolution of gaps in noise by the rat is lost with functional decortication. Behavioral Neuroscience, 105, 33–40.Google Scholar
  11. James, W. (1899). The principles of psychology, Vol. 1. New York: Holt.Google Scholar
  12. Joubert, C. E. (1983). Subjective acceleration of time: Death anxiety and sex differences. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 57, 49–50.Google Scholar
  13. Joubert, C. E. (1984). Structured time and subjective acceleration of time. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 59, 335–336.Google Scholar
  14. Kausler, D. H. (1982). Experimental psychology and human aging. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  15. Keele, S. W. (1987). Sequencing and timing in skilled perception and action: An overview. In A. Allport, D. G. MacKay, W. Prinz, & E. Scheerer (Eds.), Language perception and production: Relationships between listening, speaking, reading, and writing (pp. 463–487). London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  16. Keele, S. W., Pokomy, R. A., Corcos, D. M., & Ivry, R. (1985). Do perception and motor production share common timing mecha-nisms? A correlational analysis. Acta Psychologica, 60, 173–191.Google Scholar
  17. Klapp, S. T. (1981). Temporal compatibility in dual motor tasks II: Simultaneous articulation and hand movements. Memory & Cognition, 9, 398–401.Google Scholar
  18. LeBlanc, A. P. (1969). Time orientation and time estimation: a function of age. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 115, 187–194.Google Scholar
  19. Leibowitz, H., & Gwozdecki, J. (1967). The magnitude of the Poggendorff illusion as a function of age. Child Development, 38, 573–580.Google Scholar
  20. Leibowitz, H., & Judisch, J. M. (1967). Size constancy in older persons: A function of distance. American Journal of Psychology, 80, 294–296.Google Scholar
  21. Lemlich, R. (1975). Subjective acceleration of time with aging. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 41, 235–238.Google Scholar
  22. Levitt, H. (1971). Transformed up-down methods in psychoacoustics. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 49, 467–477.Google Scholar
  23. Lima, S. D., Hale, S., & Myerson, J. (1991). How general is general slowing? Evidence from the lexical domain. Psychology and Aging, 6, 416–425.Google Scholar
  24. Lisker, L., & Abramson, A. S. (1967). Some effects of context on voice onset time in English stops. Language and Speech, 10, 1–28.Google Scholar
  25. McCroskey, R. L. (1979). Some characteristics of temporal auditory behavior among elderly persons. In M. A. Henoch (Ed.), Aural rehabilitation for the elderly (pp. 149–167). New York: Grune and Stratton.Google Scholar
  26. McCroskey, R. L. (1984). Wichita auditory fusion test. Tulsa, OK: Modern Education Corporation.Google Scholar
  27. McCroskey, R. L., & Kasten, R. N. (1980). Assessment of central auditory processing. In R. R. Rupp & K. G. Stockdell (Eds.), Speech protocols in audiology (pp. 339–389). New York: Grune and Stratton.Google Scholar
  28. Michon, J. A. (1985). The compleat time experiencer. In J. A. Michon & J. L. Jackson (Eds.), Time, mind, and behavior (pp. 21–52). Berlin: Springer.Google Scholar
  29. Peters, M. (1977). Simultaneous performance of two motor activities: The factor of timing. Neuropsychologia, 15, 461–465.Google Scholar
  30. Prinz, W. (1987). Ideo-motor action. In H. Heuer & A. F. Sanders (Eds.), Perspectives on perception and action (pp. 47–76). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  31. Rammsayer, T. (1989 a). Dopaminergic and serotoninergic influence on duration discrimination and vigilance. Pharmacopsychiatry, 22, Suppl., 39–43.Google Scholar
  32. Rammsayer, T. (1989 b). Is there a common dopaminergic basis of time perception and reaction time? Neuropsychobiology, 21, 37–42.Google Scholar
  33. Rammsayer, T. (1990). Temporal discrimination in schizophrenic and affective disorders: evidence for a dopamine-dependent internal clock. International Journal of Neuroscience, 53, 111–120.Google Scholar
  34. Rammsayer, T. H. (1992). Die Wahrnehmung kurzer Zeitdauern. [Perception of brief time intervals]. Munster, - New York: Waxmann.Google Scholar
  35. Rammsayer, T. H., & Lima, S. D. (1991). Duration discrimination of filled and empty auditory intervals: Cognitive and perceptual factors. Perception & Psychophysics, 50, 565–574.Google Scholar
  36. Robin, D. A., & Royer, F. L. (1989). Age-related changes in auditory temporal processing. Psychology and Aging, 4, 144–149.Google Scholar
  37. Salthouse, T. A. (1985). Speed of behavior and its implications for cognition. In J. E. Birren, & K. W. Schaie (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of aging (pp. 400–426). New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold.Google Scholar
  38. Salthouse, T. A., Wright, R., & Ellis, C. L. (1979). Adult age and the rate of an internal clock. Journal of Gerontology, 34, 53–57.Google Scholar
  39. Sturr, J. F., Church, K. L., & Taub, H. A. (1988). Temporal summation functions for detection of sine-wave gratings in young and older adults. Vision Research, 28, 1247–1253.Google Scholar
  40. Surwillo, W. W. (1964). Age and the perception of short intervals of time. Journal of Gerontology, 19, 322–324.Google Scholar
  41. Treisman, M. (1963). Temporal discrimination and the indifference interval: Implication for a model of the “internal clock.” Psychological Monographs, 77, 1–31.Google Scholar
  42. Treisman, M., Faulkner, A., Naish, P. L. N., & Brogan, D. (1990). The internal clock: Evidence for a temporal oscillator underlying time perception with some estimates of its characteristic frequency. Perception, 19, 705–743.Google Scholar
  43. Walker, J. (1977). Time estimation and total subjective time. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 44, 527–532.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 1993

Authors and Affiliations

  • Thomas H. Rammsayer
    • 1
  • Susan D. Lima
    • 2
  • Wolfgang H. Vogel
    • 3
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of GiessenGiessenGermany
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyUniversity of Wisconsin-MilwaukeeMilwaukeeUSA
  3. 3.Department of Pharmacology, Thomas Jefferson UniversityThomas Jefferson UniversityPhiladelphiaUSA

Personalised recommendations