Skip to main content
Log in

The amendment of large-magnitude aiming-movement errors

  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Summary

The amendment of large-magnitude errors is discussed with particular reference to the correction of directional errors. It is argued that the correction of a large-magnitude error may be understood as a double-step tracking situation in which the second step reflects an artificially induced error signal. At issue is how the motor system may respond to such stimuli in rapid succession. The temporal integration of error stimuli predicts an initial response which reflects the weighted average of the step positions. This approach, however, does not explain the marked increase in peak velocity for the corrective response, compared to an equivalent single-step response. Alternatively, it has been argued that the initial response is initiated as if it were a single-step response to the initial step position and is subsequently amended. The superposition hypothesis argued that the two responses are planned in parallel and overlap in time, to be superimposed one on the other. This hypothesis does not explain changes in the direction of a double-step response at its initiation. The braking hypothesis argues that the initial response is halted and a corrective response initiated as rapidly as possible. This approach cannot explain changes in the slope of an ATF as a function of the second target-step amplitude. A model of double-step tracking is proposed which integrates the temporal integration and braking hypotheses. Since the braking of the initial response would involve the application of large forces, it is argued that braking is facilitated by the temporal integration of step stimuli. The corrective response is then implemented as rapidly as possible. The implications of these findings to the understanding of directional errors is discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Angel, R. W. (1976). Efference copy in the control of movement. Neurology, 26, 1164–1168.

    Google Scholar 

  • Angel, R. W., Garland, H., & Fischler, M. (1971). Tracking errors amended without visual feedback. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 89, 422–424.

    Google Scholar 

  • Angel, R. W., & Higgins, J. R. (1969). Correction of false moves in pursuit tracking. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 82, 185–187.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, N. C., & Glencross, D. J. (1988a). The double step analysis of rapid manual aiming movements. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 40A, 299–322.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, N. C., & Glencross, D. J. (1988b). The amplitude transition function and the manual and oculomotor control systems. Psychological Bulletin, 104, 363–372.

    Google Scholar 

  • Becker, W.,. & Jurgens, R. (1975). Saccadic reactions to double-step stimuli: Evidence for model feedback and continuous information uptake. In G. Lennerstrand & P. Bach-y-Rica (Eds.), Basic mechanisms of ocular motility and their clinical implications (pp. 111–119). Oxford: Pergamon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Becker, W., & Jürgens, R. (1979). An analysis of the saccadic system by means of double step stimuli. Vision Research, 19, 967–983.

    Google Scholar 

  • Becker, W., King, W. M., Fuchs, A. F., Jurgens, R., Johanson, G., & Kornhuber, H. H. (1981). Accuracy of goal directed saccades and mechanisms of error correction. In A. F. Fuchs & W. Becker (Eds.), Progress in Oculomotor Research (pp. 29–37). Elsevier, North-Holland: Amsterdam.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beggs, W. D. A., & Howarth, C. I. (1970). Movement control in a repetitive motor task. Nature, 225, 752–753.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bertelson, P. (1966). Central intermittency twenty years later. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 153–163.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carlow, T., Dell' Osso, L. F., Troost, B. T., Daroff, R. B., & Birkett, J. E. (1975). Saccadic eye movement latencies to multi-model stimuli. Intersubject variability and temporal efficiency. Vision Research, 15, 1257–1263.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carlton, L. G. (1981a). Processing visual feedback information for movement control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human perception and Performance, 7, 1019–1030.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carlton, L. G. (1981b). Visual information: The control of aiming movements. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 33A, 87–93.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, R. H. S. (1977). Movements of the eyes. London: Pion.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chernikoff, R., & Taylor, F. V. (1952). Reaction time to kinaesthetic stimulation resulting from sudden arm displacement. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 43, 1–8.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooke, J. D., & Diggles, V. A. (1984). Rapid error correction during human arm movements: Evidence for central monitoring. Journal of Motor Behaviour, 16, 348–363.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deubel, H., Wolf, W., & Hauske, G. (1984). The evaluation of the oculomotor error signal. In A. G. Gale & F. Johnson (Eds.), Theoretical and applied aspects of eye movement research (pp. 55–62). Amsterdam: Elsevier/North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elliot, D., & Allard, F. (1985). The utilization of visual feedback information during rapid pointing movements. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 37A, 407–425.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fel'dman, A. G. (1980a). Superposition of motor programs — 1. Rhythmic forearm movements in man. Neuroscience, 5, 81–90.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fel'dman, A. G. (1980b). Superposition of motor programs — 2. Rapid forearm flexion in man. Neuroscience, 5, 91–95.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fel'dman, A. G. (1986). Once more on the equilibrium-point hypothesis (-model) for motor control. Journal of Motor Behaviour, 18, 17–54.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flash, T., & Henis, E. (1991). Arm trajectory modification during reaching towards visual targets. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 3, 220–230.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flash, T., Hems, E., Inzelberg, R., & Korczyn, A. D. (1992). Timing and sequencing of human arm trajectories: Normal and abnormal motor behaviour. Human Movement Science, 11, 83–100.

    Google Scholar 

  • Georgopoulos, A. P. (1986). On reaching. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 9, 147–170.

    Google Scholar 

  • Georgopoulos, A. P., Kalaska, J. F., & Massey, J. T. (1981). Spatial trajectories and reaction times of aimed movements: Effects of practice, uncertainty and change in target location. Journal of Neurophysiology, 46, 725–743.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibbs, C. B. (1965). Probability learning in step-input tracking. British Journal of Psychology, 56, 233–242.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gielen, C. C. A. M., Van den Heuvel, P. J. M., & Dernier van der gon, J. J. (1984). Modification of muscle activation patterns during fast goal directed arm movements. Journal of Motor Behaviour, 16, 2–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottsdanker, R. (1966). The effect of superseding signals. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 236–249.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottsdanker, R. (1973). Psychological refractoriness and the organization of step tracking responses. Perception & Psychophysics, 14, 60–70.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guthrie, B. L., Porter, J. D., & Sparks, D. L. (1983). Corollary discharge provides accurate eye position information to the oculomotor system. Science, 221, 1193–1195.

    Google Scholar 

  • Higgins, J. R., & Angel, R. W. (1970). Correction of tracking errors without sensory feedback. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 84, 412–416.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jaeger, R. J., Agarwal, G. C., & Gottlieb, G. L. (1979). Directional errors of movement and their correction in a discrete tracking task. Journal of Motor Behavior, 11, 123–133.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keele, S. W., & Posner, M. I. (1968). Processing of visual feedback in rapid movement. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 77, 155–158.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelso, J. A. S., & Stelmach, G. E. (1976). Central and peripheral mechanisms in motor control. In G. E. Stelmach (Ed.), Motor control: Issues and trends (pp. 1–46). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Massey, J. T., Schwartz, A. B., & Georgopoulos, A. P. (1986). On information processing and performing a movement sequence. Experimental Brain Research Suppl., 15, 242–251.

    Google Scholar 

  • Megaw, E. D. (1972). Direction and extent uncertainty in step-input tracking. Journal of Motor Behaviour, 4, 171–186.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morris, R. (1981). Error correction paradigms: A possible confounding. In G. C. Roberts & D. M. Landers (Eds.), Psychology of motor behaviour. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, D. A. (1973). Models of the saccadic eye movement control system. Kybernetik, 14, 71–83.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, R. A. (1975). A schema theory of discrete motor skill learning. Psychological Review, 82, 225–260.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, R. A., & Gordon, G. B. (1977). Errors in motor responding, “rapid” corrections, and false anticipations. Journal of Motor Behaviour, 9, 101–111.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, W. M., & Bowen, K. F. (1980). The effects of delayed and displaced visual feedback on motor control. Journal of Motor Behavior, 8, 43–58.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Sonderen, J. F., Denier van der Gon, J. J., & Gielen, C. C. A. M. (1988). Conditions determining early modification of motor programs in response to change in target location. Experimental Brain Research, 71, 320–328.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodworth, R. S. (1899). The accuracy of voluntary movement. Psychological Review, 3 (2, Whole No. 13).

  • Zelaznik, H. N., Hawkins, B., & Kisselburgh, L. (1983). Rapid visual feedback processing in single-aiming movements. Journal of Motor Behavior, 15, 217–236.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Barrett, N.C. The amendment of large-magnitude aiming-movement errors. Psychol. Res 55, 148–155 (1993). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00419647

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00419647

Keywords

Navigation