Advertisement

Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology

, Volume 15, Issue 1, pp 39–43 | Cite as

Homing behavior of wood thrushes (Hylocichla mustelina)

A radio tracking study
  • Kenneth P. Able
  • William F. Gergits
  • Jeffrey D. Cherry
  • Scott B. Terrill
Article

Summary

Using the wood thrush (Hylocichla mustelina) as an example of a typical nocturnal migrant, we employed radio telemetry to follow breeding birds during the course of homing movements following displacements. Seven thrushes were displaced over distances of 6.5 to 17.3 km in a variety of directions from their nesting territories. The thrushes moved in a series of short flights (mean = 2.1 km) performed primarily at dawn. Consequently, the birds took several days to home from even these relatively short displacements. Thrushes flew under clear and solid overcast skies and even in light rain. The pooled individual flights of the birds were significantly oriented in the homeward direction (Fig. 2). Their orientation relative to home did not improve significantly as they progressed toward the goal. Three of the birds were documented to have returned to their home territories. The detailed tracks of the birds preclude the possibility that they homed by random search.

Keywords

Random Search Breeding Bird Light Rain Radio Telemetry Homing Behavior 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Able KP (1980) Mechanisms of orientation, navigation and homing. In: Gauthreaux SA (ed) Animal migration, orientation, and navigation. Academic Press, New York, pp 325–333Google Scholar
  2. Baker MC, Sherman GL, Theimer TC, Bradley DC (1982) Population biology of White-crowned Sparrows: residence time and local movements of juveniles. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 11:133–137Google Scholar
  3. Balen JH van (1979) Observations on the post-fledging dispersal of the Pied Flycatcher, Ficedula hypoleuca. Ardea 67:134–137Google Scholar
  4. Batschelet E (1981) Circular statistics in biology. Academic Press, LondonGoogle Scholar
  5. Berndt R, Sternberg H (1969) Über Begriffe, Ursachen und Auswirkungen der Dispersion bei Vögeln. Vogelwelt 90:41–53Google Scholar
  6. Billings SM (1968) Homing in Leach's Petrel. Auk 85:36–43Google Scholar
  7. Cochran WW (1972) Long-distance tracking of birds. In: Galler SR, Schmidt-Koenig K, Jacobs GJ, Belleville RE (eds) Animal orientation and navigation. NASA SP-262, Washington, DC, pp 39–59Google Scholar
  8. Downhower JF, Windsor DM (1971) Use of landmarks in orientation by Bank Swallows. BioScience 21:570–572Google Scholar
  9. Gould JL (1982) The map sense of pigeons. Nature 296:205–211Google Scholar
  10. Griffin DR (1940) Homing experiments with Leach's Petrels. Auk 57:61–74Google Scholar
  11. Griffin DR (1955) Bird navigation. In: Wolfson A (ed) Recent studies in avian biology. University of Illinois Press, Urbana, Ill, pp 154–197Google Scholar
  12. Hirth DH, Hester AE, Greeley F (1969) Dispersal and flocking of marked young robins (Turdus m. migratorius) after fledging. Bird-Banding 40:208–215Google Scholar
  13. Keeton WT (1974) The orientational and navigational basis of homing in birds. Adv Study Behav 5:47–132Google Scholar
  14. Kenyon KW, Rice DW (1958) Homing of Laysan Albatrosses. Condor 60:3–6Google Scholar
  15. Matthews GVT (1968) Bird navigation, 2nd edn. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  16. Mewaldt LR, Archibald H (1966) Some home-range measurements of groundfeeding birds at PRBO. Annu Rep Point Reyes Bird Observatory 1966:13–20Google Scholar
  17. Nastase AJ (1982) Orientation and homing ability of the Barn Swallow. J Field Ornithol 53:15–21Google Scholar
  18. Nolan V (1978) The ecology and behavior of the Prairie Warbler Dendroica discolor. Ornithol Monogr 26Google Scholar
  19. Raim A (1978) A radio transmitter attachment for small passerine birds. Bird-Banding 49:226–332Google Scholar
  20. Rüppell W (1934) Heimfindeversuche mit Rauchschwalben (Hirundo rustica) and Mehlschwalben (Delichon urbica) von H. Warnat (Berlin-Charlottenburg). Vogelzug 5:161–166Google Scholar
  21. Rüppell W (1936) Heimfindeversuche mit Staren und Schwalben, 1935. J Ornithol 84:180–198Google Scholar
  22. Rüppell W (1937) Heimfindeversuche mit Staren, Rauchschwalben, Wendehalsen, Rotrückenwürgern und Habichten (1936). J Ornithol 85:120–135Google Scholar
  23. Sargent TD (1962) A study of homing in the Bank Swallow (Raparia riparia). Auk 79:234–246Google Scholar
  24. Schmidt-Koenig K (1979) Avian orientation and navigation. Academic Press, LondonGoogle Scholar
  25. Southern WE (1959) Homing in Purple Martins. Wilson Bull 71:254–261Google Scholar
  26. Southern WE (1968) Experiments on the homing ability of Purple Martins. Living Bird 7:71–84Google Scholar
  27. Wojtusiak RJ (1948) Polish investigations on homing in birds and their orientation in space. Proc Linn Soc (Lond) 160:99–108Google Scholar
  28. Wojtusiak RJ, Ferens B (1947) Homing experiments on birds. VII. Further investigations on the velocity of swallows (Hirundo rustica L.) and on the role of memory in their orientation in space. Bull Int Acad Pol 2:135–164Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 1984

Authors and Affiliations

  • Kenneth P. Able
    • 1
  • William F. Gergits
    • 1
  • Jeffrey D. Cherry
    • 1
  • Scott B. Terrill
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of BiologyState University of New York at AlbanyAlbanyUSA

Personalised recommendations