Skip to main content
Log in

The roles of imagery, language, and metamemory in cross-modal transfer in children

  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Summary

The relative strengths of association of imagery, language, and metamemory to cross-modal transfer were evaluated in 5-year-olds and 7-year-olds. Children were administered either tactual-visual or visual-tactual cross-modal transfer tasks, as well as two visual imagery tasks (spatial recall and mental rotation), two language competence tasks (vocabulary and word usage), metamemory questions, and a general intelligence task. With intelligence and handedness controlled, only visual imagery related significantly to cross-modal transfer. These findings suggest that mental imagery, as opposed to language competence or metamemory, is a principal component of cross-modal transfer, and they are consistent with the observation that preverbal human infants and infrahuman primates exhibit cross-modal transfer.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abravanel, E. (1968). The development of intersensory patterning with regard to selected spatial dimensions. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 33.

  • Abravanel, E. (1972). Short-term memory for shape information processed intra-and intermodally at three ages. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 35, 419–425.

    Google Scholar 

  • Birch, H. G., & Lefford, A. (1963). Intersensory development in children. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 28.

  • Birch, H. G., & Lefford, A. (1967). Visual differentiation, intersensory integration, and voluntary motor control. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 32.

  • Blank, M., Altman, D. L., & Bridger, W. H. (1968). Crossmodal transfer of form discrimination in preschool children. Psychonomic Science, 10, 51–52.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blank, M., & Bridger, W. H. (1964). Cross-modal transfer in nursery school children. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 58, 277–282.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borkowski, J. G., & Cavanaugh, J. C. (1979). Maintenance and generalization of skills and strategies by the retarded. In N. R. Ellis (Ed.), Handbook of mental deficiency: Psychological theory and research. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bornstein, M. H. (1984). A descriptive taxonomy of psychological categories used by infants. In C. Sophian (Ed.), Origins of cognitive skills (pp 313–338) Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. S., Olver, R. R., & Greenfield, P. M. (1966). Studies in cognitive growth. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bryant, P. E. (1968). Comments on the design of developmental studies of cross-modal matching and cross-modal transfer. Cortex, 4, 127–137.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bryant, P., Jones, P., Claxton, V., & Perkins, G. M. (1972). Recognition of shapes across modalities by infants. Nature, 240, 303–304.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butter, E. J., & Zung, B. J. (1970). A developmental investigation of the effect of sensory modality on form recognition in children. Developmental Psychology, 3, 276.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cavanaugh, J. C., & Borkowski, J. G. (1980). Searching for metamemory-memory connections: A developmental study. Developmental Psychology, 16, 441–453.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cavanaugh, J. C., & Pearlmutter, M. (1982). Metamemory: A critical examination. Child Development, 53, 11–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Childs, M. K., & Polich, J. M. (1979). Developmental differences in mental rotation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 27, 339–351.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J., & Cohen, P. (1975). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cranney, J., & Ashton, R. (1982). Tactile spatial ability: Lateralized performance of deaf and hearing age groups. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 34, 123–134.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davenport, R. K., Rogers, C. M., & Russell, I. S. (1973). Crossmodal perception in apes. Neuropsychologia, 11, 21–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dean, A. L., & Scherzer, E. (1982). A comparison of reaction time and drawing measures of mental rotation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 34, 20–37.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dirks, J., & Neisser, U. (1977). Memory for objects in real scenes: The development of recognition and recall. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 23, 315–328.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1965). Peabody picture vocabulary test. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, J. H. (1985). Cognitive development (2nd ed.). Englewood-Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Friedes, D. (1974). Human information processing and sensory modality: Cross-modal functions, information complexity, memory, and deficit. Psychological Bulletin, 81, 284–310.

    Google Scholar 

  • Galin, D., Johnstone, J., Nakell, L., & Herron, J. (1979). Development of the capacity for tactile information transfer between hemispheres in normal children. Science, 204, 1330–1334.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garvill, J., & Molander, B. (1971). Verbal mediation effects in cross-modal transfer. British Journal of Psychology, 62, 449–547.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, E. J. (1969). Principles of perceptual learning and development. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodnow, J. J. (1971). Eye and hand: Differential memory and its effect on matching. Neuropsychologia, 9, 89–95.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottfried, A. W., & Rose, S. A. (1980). Tactile recognition memory in infants. Child Development, 51, 69–74.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gunderson, V. M. (1983). Development of cross-modal recognition in infant pigtail monkeys (Macaca nemestrina). Developmental Psychology, 19, 398–404.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halsey, W. D. (1982). Macmillan dictionary for children. New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jarvis, M. J., & Ettlinger, G. (1977). Cross-modal recognition in chimpanzees and monkeys. Neuropsychologia, 15, 499–506.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, B., & Robinson, T. (1973). Sensory integration in normal and retarded children. Developmental Psychology, 9, 178–182.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kail, R., Pelligrino, J., & Carter, P. (1980). Developmental changes in mental rotation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 29, 102–116.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koen, F. (1971). Verbal mediators in cross-modal form discrimination. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 25, 103–110.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kosslyn, S. M. (1980). Image and mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kosslyn, S. N., Brunn, J., Cave, K. R., & Wallach, R. W. (1984). Individual differences in mental imagery ability: A computational analysis. Cognition, 18, 195–243.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kreutzer, M. A., Leonard, C., & Flavell, J. H. (1975). An interview study of children's knowledge about memory. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 40.

  • Marmor, G. S. (1975). Development of kinetic images: When does the child first represent movement in mental images? Cognitive Psychology, 7, 548–559.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marmor, G. S. (1977). Mental rotation and number conservation: Are they related? Developmental Psychology, 13, 320–325.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGurk, H., & Power, R. P. (1980). Intermodal coordination in young children: Vision and touch. Developmental Psychology, 16, 679–680.

    Google Scholar 

  • Millar, S. (1971). Visual and haptic cue utilization by school children: The recognition of visual and haptic stimuli presented separately and together. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 12, 88–94.

    Google Scholar 

  • Millar, S. (1972a). The development of visual and kinaesthetic judgements of distance. British Journal of Psychology, 63, 271–282.

    Google Scholar 

  • Millar, S. (1972b). Effects of interpolated tasks on latency and accuracy of intramodal and cross-modal shape recognition by children. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 96, 170–175.

    Google Scholar 

  • Milner, A. D., & Bryant, P. E. (1970). Cross-modal matching by young children. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 71, 453–458.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh Inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97–113.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1968). The psychology of the child. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1971). Mental imagery in the child. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rose, S. A. (1980). Enhancing visual recognition memory in preterm infants. Developmental Psychology, 16, 85–92.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rose, S. A., Blank, M. S., & Bridger, W. H. (1972). Intermodal and intramodal retention of visual and tactual information in young children. Developmental Psychology, 6, 482–486.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rose, S. A., Gottfried, A. W., & Bridger, W. H. (1979). Effects of haptic cues on visual recognition memory in fullterm and preterm infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 2, 55–67.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rose, S. A., Gottfried, A. W., & Bridger, W. H. (1983). Infant's cross-modal transfer from solid objects to their graphic representations. Child Development, 15, 687–690.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rudel, R. G., & Teuber, H.-L. (1964). Cross-modal transfer of shape discrimination by children. Neuropsychologia, 2, 1–8.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rudel, R. G., & Teuber, H.-L. (1971). Pattern recognition within and across sensory modalities in normal and brain-injured children. Neuropsychologia, 9, 389–399.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ruff, H. A., & Kohler, C. J. (1978). Tactual-visual transfer in 6-month-old infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 1, 259–264.

    Google Scholar 

  • Satz, P., Taylor, H. G., Friel, J., & Fletcher, J. (1978). Some developmental and predictive precursors of reading disabilities: A six year follow-up. In A. L. Benton, & D. Pearl (Eds.), Dyslexia: An appraisal of current knowledge. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Von Wright, J. M. (1970). Cross-modal transfer and sensory equivalence — a review. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 11, 21–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1967). Wechsler preschool and primary school intelligence. New York: Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1974). Wechsler intelligence scales for children-revised. New York: Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • White, S. (1970). Some general outlines of the matrix of developmental changes between five and seven years. Bulletin of the Orton Society, 20, 41–57.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Stoltz-Loike, M., Bornstein, M.H. The roles of imagery, language, and metamemory in cross-modal transfer in children. Psychol. Res 49, 63–68 (1987). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00309204

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00309204

Keywords

Navigation