Skip to main content
Log in

Do gold Midas cichlid fish win fights because of their color, or because they lack normal coloration?

A logistic solution

  • Published:
Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Summary

Gold morphs (G) of the polychromatic Midas cichlid, Cichlasoma citrinellum, dominate those of normal coloration (N). The adults vary greatly in weight because they continue to grow after reaching sexual maturity; relative weight is an important determinant of dominance. The first objective here was to characterize dominance in relation to color and to relative size by staging a large number of encounters between fish of various weights. Logistic curves were fitted to the data to analyze the relationship between relative size and winning. The baseline curves for N vs N and G vs G (Fig. 1, Table 1) did not differ (Table 2). The shape of the curve for N vs G also was not different from that of the baseline curves. But the curve for N vs G was shifted such that N must weigh 116% of G for the probability of winning to be 0.50.

The dominance effect of G stems directly from their color, not from the absence of normal markings or from genetically coupled aggressiveness. When G were matched against white morphs (W) the logistic curve was displaced such that W must be 117% the weight of G to enjoy a 50% chance of winning, just as with N vs G. In N vs W, the chance of either winning was 50% when they were equal in weight, thus just as in the baseline curves for N vs N and G vs G.

Absolute size played a role. Large mature fish had a higher logistic coefficient, indicating greater sensitivity to differences in relative weight, than did small immature fish (Table 3).

Data were also gathered from some of the contests on the consequences of winning and losing. Losers were often forced to the surface and suffered more splits in their fins. However, when G lost to N many G were tolerated near the bottom (Table 4). On average, G losers had fewer splits in their fins than did the N winners. W losers were tolerated near the bottom, and W winners and losers suffered litte damage, suggesting the colorless pattern W provokes fewer attacks (Tables 4–6).

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Baerends GP, Baerends-van Roon JM (1950) An introduction to the study of the ethology of cichlid fishes. Behaviour (Suppl) 1:1–242

    Google Scholar 

  • Barlow GW (1973) Competition between color morphs of the polychromatic Midas cichlid Cichlasoma citrinellum. Science 179:806–807

    Google Scholar 

  • Barlow GW (1976) The Midas cichlid in Nicaragua. In: Thorson TB (ed) Investigations of the ichthyofauna of Nicaraguan Lakes. School of Life Sciences. University of Nebraska, Lincoln (Nebraska), pp 333–358

    Google Scholar 

  • Barlow GW (1983) The benefits of being gold: behavioral consequences of polychromatism in the Midas cichlid, Cichlasoma citrinellum. Env Biol Fish 8:235–247

    Google Scholar 

  • Barlow GW, Ballin PJ (1976) Predicting and assessing dominance from size and colouration in the polychromatic Midas cichlid. Anim Behav 24:793–813

    Google Scholar 

  • Barlow GW, McKaye KR (1982) A comparison of feeding, spacing, and aggression in color morphs of the Midas cichlid. II. After 24 hours without food. Behaviour 80:127–142

    Google Scholar 

  • Barlow GW, Wallach SJ (1976) Colour and levels of aggression in the Midas cichlid. Anim Behav 24:814–817

    Google Scholar 

  • Barlow GW, Bauer DH, McKaye KR (1975) A comparison of feeding, spacing, and aggression in color morphs of the Midas cichlid. I. Food continously present. Behaviour 54:72–96

    Google Scholar 

  • Barlow GW, Rogers W, Cappeto RV (1977) Incompatibility and assortative mating in the Midas cichlid. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 2:49–59

    Google Scholar 

  • Berkson J (1953) A statistically precise and relatively simple method of estimating the bioassay with quantal response, based on the logistic function. J Am Stat Assoc 48:565–599

    Google Scholar 

  • Braddock JC (1945) Some aspects of the dominance-subordination relationship in the fish Platypoecilus maculatus. Physiol Zool 18:176–195

    Google Scholar 

  • Cole L (1962) A closed sequential test design for toleration experiments. Ecology 43:749–753

    Google Scholar 

  • Hammerstein P, Parker GA (1982) The asymmetric war of attrition. J Theor Biol 96:647–682

    Google Scholar 

  • Knoke D, Burke PJ (1980) Log-linear models. Sage, Beverly Hills London (Quantitative Applications in the Social Sciences, No. 07/020, pp 1–79)

  • Luria SM, Kinney JS (1970) Underwater vision. Science 167:1454–1461

    Google Scholar 

  • Maynard Smith J (1974) The theory of games and the evolution of animal conflicts. J Theor Biol 47:209–222

    Google Scholar 

  • Maynard Smith J, Price GR (1973) The logic of animal conflict. Nature 246:15–18

    Google Scholar 

  • McFadden D (1974) Conditional logic analysis of qualitative choice behavior. In: Zarembka P (ed) Frontiers in econometrics. Academic Press, New York, pp 105–142

    Google Scholar 

  • Parker GA (1974) Assessment strategy and the evolution of fighting behaviour. J Theor Biol 47:223–343

    Google Scholar 

  • Parker GA, Rubenstein DI (1981) Role assessment, reserve strategy, and acquisition of information in asymmetric animal conflicts. Anim Behav 29:221–240

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Barlow, G.W. Do gold Midas cichlid fish win fights because of their color, or because they lack normal coloration?. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 13, 197–204 (1983). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00299923

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00299923

Keywords

Navigation