Advertisement

Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology

, Volume 20, Issue 3, pp 179–185 | Cite as

The role of learning in the mating biology of a sweat bee Lasioglossum zephyrum (Hymenoptera: Halictidae)

  • William T. Wcislo
Article

Summary

Results from experiments on the role of learning in the mating biology of a sweat bee, Lasioglossum zephyrum (Hymenoptera: Halictidae), are described in this paper. Male learning of individual female odors is important in natural populations (Table 1), as has been shown in the laboratory. Four other hypotheses are unlikely to account for the observed behavior: (1) Female odors dissipate rapidly; (2) Males learn and then avoid the study area; (3) Males or females produce repellents which are effective against other males; or (4) males recognize their own odor on previously-contacted females, which they subsequently avoid. Regarding questions of “male preference” and “optimal outbreeding,” Tables 2 and 3 show there are no consistent preferences for more novel or less novel female odors.

Keywords

Natural Population Male Preference Consistent Preference Female Odor Male Learning 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Alcock J, Barrows EM, Gordh G, Hubbard LJ, Kirkendall LL, Pyle D, Ponder TL, Zalom FG (1978) The ecology and evolution of male reproductive behaviour in the bees and wasps. Zool J Linn Soc, London, 64:293–326bGoogle Scholar
  2. Alexander RD, Borgia G (1979) On the origin and bases of the male-female phenomenon. In: Blum MS, Blum NA (eds) Sexual selection and reproductive competition in insects. Academic Press, London, pp 417–440Google Scholar
  3. Assem J van den, Jachmann J, Simbolotti F (1980) Courtship behaviour of Nasonia vitripennis (Hymenoptera: Pteromalidae): some qualitative experimental evidence for the role of pheromones. Behaviour 75:301–307Google Scholar
  4. Barrows EM (1975a) Individually distinctive odors in an invertebrate. Behav Biol 15:57–64Google Scholar
  5. Barrows EM (1975b) Mating behavior in halictine bees (Hymenoptera: Halictidae). III. Copulatory behavior and olfactory communication. Insectes Soc 22:307–332Google Scholar
  6. Barrows EM, Bell WJ, Michener CD (1975) Individual odor differences and their social functions in insects. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 72:2824–2828Google Scholar
  7. Bateson PPG (1973) Preference for familiarity and novelty: a model for the simultaneous development of both. J Theor Biol 41:249–259Google Scholar
  8. Bateson P (1983) Optimal outbreeding. In: Bateson P (ed) Mate choice. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 367–377Google Scholar
  9. Batra SWT (1966) The life cycle and behavior of the primitively social bee Lasioglossum zephyrum (Halictidae). Univ Kansas Sci Bull 46:359–423Google Scholar
  10. Cane JH, Tengö J (1981) Pheromonal cues direct mate seeking behavior of male Colletes cunicularis (Hymenoptera: Colletidae). J Chem Ecol 7:427–436Google Scholar
  11. Clayton DA (1978) Socially facilitated behavior. Q Rev Biol 53:373–392Google Scholar
  12. Darwin C (1871) The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex. Princeton University Press, Princeton, vol I, pp 423Google Scholar
  13. Eberhard WG (1985) Sexual selection and animal genitalia. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, pp 244Google Scholar
  14. Fisher RA (1958) The genetical theory of natural selection (2nd ed). Dover Publications, New York, pp 291Google Scholar
  15. Fletcher DJC, Michener CD (eds) (1987) Kin recognition in animals, Wiley & Sons, Ltd, Chichester, EnglandGoogle Scholar
  16. Greenberg L (1979) Genetic component of bee odor in kin recognition. Science 206:1095–1097Google Scholar
  17. Greenberg L (1982) Persistent habituation to female odor by male sweat bees Lasioglossum zephyrum (Hymenoptera: Halictidae). J Kansas Entomol Soc 55:525–531Google Scholar
  18. Hepper PG (1986) Kin recognition: function and mechanisms A review. Biol Rev 61:63–93Google Scholar
  19. Hölldobler B, Michener CD (1980) Mechanisms of identification and discrimination in social Hymenoptera. In: Markl H (ed) Evolution of social behavior: Hypotheses and empirical tests. VCH, Weinheim, pp 35–58Google Scholar
  20. Knoppien P (1985) Rare male mating advantage: a review. Biol Rev 60:81–117Google Scholar
  21. Kukuk P (1985) Evidence for an antiaphrodisiac in the sweat bee Lasioglossum (Dialictus) zephyrum. Science 227:656–657Google Scholar
  22. Kukuk PF, Decelles P (1986) Behavioral evidence of population structure in Lasioglossum zephyrum (Hymenoptera: Halictidae): female dispersion patterns. Behav Biol Sociobiol 19:233–239Google Scholar
  23. Kukuk P, Eickwort GC, Wesley FR (1985) Mate-seeking behavior of Dufourea novaeangliae (Hymenoptera: Halictidae: Dufoureinae): the effects of resource distribution. J Kansas Entomol Soc 58:142–150Google Scholar
  24. Kullenberg B (1956) Field experiments with chemical sexual attractants on aculeate Hymenoptera males. J Zool Bidrag Uppsala 31:254–353Google Scholar
  25. Lloyd JE (1980) Sexual selection: individuality, identification, and recognition in a bumblebee and other insects. Fla Entomol 64:89–118Google Scholar
  26. Michener CD (1974) The social behavior of the bees. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, pp 404Google Scholar
  27. Michener CD, Breed MD, Bell WJ (1979) Seasonal cycles, nests, and social behavior of some Colombian halictine bees. Rev Biol Trop 27:13–34Google Scholar
  28. Michener CD, Smith BH (1987) Kin recognition in primitively eusocial insects. In: Fletcher DJC, Michener CD (eds) Kin recognition in animals. John Wiley & Sons, Ltd, Chichester, England, pp 209–242Google Scholar
  29. Mostofsky D (ed) (1965) Stimulus generalization. Stanford: Stanford University PressGoogle Scholar
  30. Robson GC, Richards OW (1936) The variation of animals in nature. Longmans, Green & Co, London, pp 425Google Scholar
  31. Silverstein RM (1984) Chemistry of insect communication. In: Lewis T (ed) Insect communication. Academic Press, New York, pp 105–121Google Scholar
  32. Smith BH (1983) Recognition of female kin by male bees through olfactory signals. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 80:4551–4553Google Scholar
  33. Smith BH (1984) Pheromonally based kin and specific recognition in the Hymenoptera. PhD thesis, University of Kansas, Lawrence KansasGoogle Scholar
  34. Smith BH, Carlson RH, Frazier J (1985) Identification and bioassay of macrocyclic lactone sex pheromone of the halictine bee Lasioglossum zephyrum (Hymenoptera: Halictidae). J Chem Ecol 11:1447–1456Google Scholar
  35. Smith BH, Wenzel JW (in press) Pheromonal covariation and kinship in the social bee Lasioglossum zephyrum (Hymenoptera: Halictidae). J Chem EcolGoogle Scholar
  36. Sokal RR, Rohlf FJ (1981) Biometry. WH Freeman & Co, San Francisco, pp 859Google Scholar
  37. Thornhill R, Alcock J (1983) The evolution of insect mating systems. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, pp 547Google Scholar
  38. Trivers RL (1972) Parental investment and sexual selection. In: Campbell B (ed) Sexual selection and the descent of man 1871–1971. Aldine, Chicago, pp 136–179Google Scholar
  39. West-Eberhard MJ (1983) Sexual selection, social competition, and speciation. Q Rev Biol 58:155–183Google Scholar
  40. West-Eberhard MJ (1984) Sexual selection, competitive communication and species-specific signals in insects. In: Lewis T (ed) Insect communication. Academic Press, New York, pp 283–324Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 1987

Authors and Affiliations

  • William T. Wcislo
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of EntomologyUniversity of KansasLawrenceUSA

Personalised recommendations