Skip to main content
Log in

Difference without dominance: Children's talk in mixed- and same-sex dyads

  • Published:
Sex Roles Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study explores whether third graders verbalize gender differences in dominance in mixed- and same-sex interaction. We tape-recorded the conversations of 43 pairs of Caucasian working-class children playing checkers in same- or mixed-sex conditions. Children appear to develop gender-differentiated speech styles. Boys brag and insult their opponents in both mixed- and same-sex conditions more often than do girls. Girls talk off-topic, interrupt, and laugh more in same-sex dyads than do boys or either, boys or girls in mixed-sex dyads. Gender differences in same-sex interaction were reflected in mixed-sex interaction. Although boys account for a larger proportion of direct requests and self-promoting speech in mixed-sex encounters, we failed to uncover substantial asymmetry in mixed-sex interaction, indicating that boys do not conversationally dominate girls in third grade. However, children were less mutually engaged in mixed-sex than in same-sex interactions, and girls especially showed less positive affect in mixed-sex dyads.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Anderson, E. S. (1984). The acquisition of sociolinguistic knowledge: Some evidence from children's verbal role play. Western Journal of Speech Communication, 48, 125–144.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aries, E., & Johnson, F. L. (1983). Close friendship in adulthood: Conversational content between same-sex friends. Sex Roles, 9, 1183–1196.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bates, E., & Silvern, L. (1977). Social adjustment and politeness in preschoolers. Journal of Communication, 27, 104–111.

    Google Scholar 

  • Becker, J. A. (1982). Children's strategic use of requests to mark and manipulate social status. In S. Kuczaj (Ed.), Language development: Language, thought and culture (Vol 2). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, R. (1973). A first language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, P., & Levinson, S. C. (1987). Politeness: Some universals in language usage. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carter, D. B. (1986). The role of peers in sex role socialization. In D. B. Carter (Ed.), Current theory and research in gender. New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carter, D. B., & Levy, G. D. (1988). Cognitive aspects of early sex role development: The influence of gender schemas on preschoolers' memories and preferences for sex-typed toys and activities. Child Development, 59, 782–792.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, H. (1979). Responding to indirect speech acts. Cognitive Psychology, 11, 430–477.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coleman, L. M. (1987). Language and the evolution of identity and self concept. In F. Kessel (Ed.), Language research and language researchers. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Courtright, J., Millar, F., & Rogers-Millar, E. (1979). Domineeringness and dominance: Replication and expansion. Communication Monographs, 46, 1979–1992.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cowan, P. A., Weber, B. A., Hoddinnot, & Klein, J. (1967). Mean length of spoken response as a function of stimulus, experimenter, and subject. Child Development, 38, 191–203.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crosby, F., & Nyquist, I. (1977). The female register: An empirical study of Lakoff's hypotheses. Language in Society, 6, 313–322.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Boer, M. (1987). Sex differences in language: Observations of dyadic conversations between members of the same-sex. In D. Brouwer, D. Haan (Eds.), Women's language, socialization and self image. The Netherlands: Foris Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eakins, B., & Eakins, G. (1976). Verbal turn-taking and exchanges in faculty dialogue. In D. L. Dubois & I. Crouch (Eds.), The sociology of the languages of American women. San Antonio, TX: Trinity University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Edelsky, C. (1977). Acquisition of an aspect of communicative competence: learning what it means to talk like a lady. In S. Ervin-Trip & Mitchell-Kernan C. (Eds.), Child discourse. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Edelsky, C. (1981). Who's got the floor? Language in Society, 10, 383–421.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ervin-Tripp, S., & Gordon, D. (1984). The development of requests. In R. L. Schiefelbusch (Ed.), Language competence: Assessment and intervention. San Diego, CA: College Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, S., Rogoff, B., & Cromer, C.C. (1981). Age segregation in children's social interaction. Developmental Psychology, 17, 399–407.

    Google Scholar 

  • Esposito, A. (1979). Sex differences in children's conversation. Language in Society, 22, 213–220.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fishman, P. (1983). Interaction: The work women do. In B. Thorne, C. Kramarae, & N. Henley (Eds.), Language, gender and society. Rowley, MA: Newbury House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foot, H. C., Chapman, A. J., & Smith, J. R. (1977). Friendship and social responsiveness in boys and girls. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 401–411.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilligan, C. (1982). In a different voice: Psychological theory and women's development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, M. H. (1980). Directive response sequences in girls' and boys' task activities. In S. McConnell-Ginet, R. Borker, and R. Furman (Eds.), Women and language in literature and society. New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, M. H., & Goodwin, C. (1987). Children's arguing. In S. U. Philips, S. Steele, & C. Tanz (Eds.), Language, gender and sex in comparative perspective. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goffman, E. (1959). The presentation of self in everyday life. New York: Doubleday.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottman, J. M., & Ringland, J. T. (1981). The analysis of dominance and bidirectionality in social development. Child Development, 81, 393–412.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hartup, W. W. (1983). Peer relations. In P. H. Mussen & E. M. Hetherington (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology (4th ed., Vol. IV). Socialization, personality, and social development. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hill, J. (1987). Women's speech in modern Mexicano. In S. U. Philips, S. Steele, & C. Tanz (Eds.), Language, gender and sex in comparative perspective. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holmes, J. (1986). Functions of “you know” in women's and men's speech. Language in Society, 22, 1–21.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ickes, W., & Barnes, R. D. (1978). Boys and girls together—and alienated: On enacting stereotyped sex roles in mixed-sex dyads. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 669–682.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jacklin, C. M., & Maccoby, E. E. (1978). Social behavior at thirty-three months in same-sex and mixed-sex dyads. Child Development, 49, 557–569.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kennedy, C. W., & Camden, C. T. (1983). A new look at interruptions. Western Journal of Speech Communication, 47, 45–58.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knight, G. P., & Chao, C. C. (1988). Gender differences in the cooperative, competitive, and individualistic social values of children. Motivation and Emotion, 13, 125–141.

    Google Scholar 

  • LaFreniere, P., Strayer, F. F., & Gauthier, R. (1984). The emergence of same-sex preferences among preschool peers: A developmental ethological perspective. Child Development, 55, 1958–1966.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, R. (1975). Language and woman's place. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Langlois, J., Gottfried, J., & Seay, B. (1973). The influence of sex of peer on the social behavior of preschool children. Developmental Psychology, 8, 93–98.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leaper, C. (1987). Agency, communion, and gender as predictors of communication style and being liked in adult male-female dyads. Sex Roles, 16, 137–149.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leaper, C. (1991). Influence and involvement in children's discourse: Age, gender and partner effects. Child Development, 62, 797–811.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lever, J. (1976). Sex differences in the games children play. Social Problems, 23, 478–483.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maccoby, E. E. (1990). Gender and relationships. American Psychologist, 45, 513–520.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maltz, D. M., & Borker, R. A. (1982). A cultural approach to male-female miscommunication. In Gumperz, J. (Ed.), Language, interaction and social identity. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCloskey, L. A. (1987). Gender and conversation: Mixing and matching styles. In D. B. Carter (Ed.), Current conceptions of sex roles and sex typing. New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • McConnell-Ginet, S., Borker, R., & Furman, N. (Eds). (1980). Women and language in literature and society. New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mueller, E. (1972). The maintenance of verbal exchanges between young children. Child Development, 43, 930–938.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mulac, A., Wiemann, J. M., Widenmann, J., & Gibson, T. W. (1988). Male/female language differences and effects in same-sex and mixed-sex dyads: The gender-linked language effect. Communication Monographs, 55, 315–335.

    Google Scholar 

  • O'Barr, W., & Atkins (1980). “Women's language” or “powerless language?” In S. McConnell-Ginet, R. Borker, & N. Furman (Eds.), Women and language in literature and society. New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Philips, S. U., Steele, S., & Tanz, C. (1987). Language, gender and sex in comparative perspective. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roopnarine, J. (1984). Sex typed socialization in mixed-age preschool classrooms. Child Development, 55, 1078–1084.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sachs, J. (1987). Preschool boys' and girls' language use in pretend play. In S. U. Philips, S. Steele, & C. Tanz (Eds.), Language, gender and sex in comparative perspective. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sacks, H., Schegloff, E. A., & Jefferson, G. (1974). A simplest systematics for the organization of turn-taking for conversation. Language, 50, 696–735.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sgan, M. L., & Pickert, S. M. (1980). Cross-sex and same-sex assertive bids in a cooperative group task. Child Development, 51, 928–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shatz, M. (1986). Communication. In P. H. Mussen & E. M. Hetherington (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology (4th Ed., Vol. 3). Cognitive Development. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thorne, B. & Henley, N. (Eds.) (1975). Language and Sex: Difference and Dominance. Rowley, MA: Newbury House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thorne, B. (1986). Girls and boys together ... but mostly apart. In W. W. Hartup & Z. Rubin (Eds.), Relationships and Development. Hillsdale NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thorne, B., Kramarae, C., & Henley, N. (Eds.). (1983). Language, gender and society. Cambridge, MA: Newbury House.

    Google Scholar 

  • von Salisch, M. (1987, April). The maintenance of affect among peers. Paper presented at the Biennial Conference of the Society for Research in Child Development, April, Baltimore, MD.

  • West, C., & Zimmerman, D. (1983). Small insults: A study of interruptions in cross-sex conversations between unacquainted persons. In B. Thorne, C. Kramarae, & N. Henley (Eds.), Language, gender and society. Cambridge, MA: Newbury House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilkinson, L. C., Lindow, J., & Chiang, C. P. (1985). Sex differences and sex segregation in students' small group communication. In L. C. Wilkinson & C. B. Marrett (Eds.), Gender influences in classroom interaction. Orlando, FL: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, J. E., Bennett, S. M., & Best, D. L. (1975). Awareness and expression of sex stereotypes in young children. Developmental Psychology. 11, 635–642.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winter, W. D., & Ferreira, A. J. (1969). Talking time as an index of intrafamilial similarity in normal and abnormal families. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 74, 575–575.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman, D., & West, C. (1975). Sex roles, interruptions and silences in conversation. In B. Thorne & N. Henley, (Eds.), Language and sex: Difference and dominance. Rowley, MA: Newbury House.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

We wish to thank the schools in southeastern Michigan that participated in this research project: Dundee Elementary, Willis Regional Elementary, and Rawsonville Elementary. We thank Audrey Ebersold, Nancy Jang, Jeffrey Jones, Beth Goodman, Tina Krugler, Helen McCloskey, Karin Nanos, Greta Rauer, Annice Siders, and Juliette Summers for their assistance in collecting, transcribing, or coding these tapes. John Simonson provided statistical and programming advice and support. The ideas presented in this paper have benefitted from discussions with Donald McCloskey, Helen McCloskey, and Marilyn Shatz. We also thank Jeff Greenberg for helpful comments on this manuscript.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

McCloskey, L.A., Coleman, L.M. Difference without dominance: Children's talk in mixed- and same-sex dyads. Sex Roles 27, 241–257 (1992). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00289927

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00289927

Keywords

Navigation