Advertisement

Experimental Brain Research

, Volume 49, Issue 1, pp 93–115 | Cite as

The orbitofrontal cortex: Neuronal activity in the behaving monkey

  • S. J. Thorpe
  • E. T. Rolls
  • S. Maddison
Article

Summary

Single unit recording of neurons in the orbitofrontal cortex of the alert rhesus monkey was used to investigate responses to sensory stimulation. 32.4% of the neurons had visual responses that had typical latencies of 100–200 ms, and 9.4% responded to gustatory inputs. Most neurons were selective, in that they responded consistently to some stimuli such as foods or aversive objects, but not to others. In a number of cases the neurons responded selectively to particular foods or aversive stimuli. However, this high selectivity could not be explained by simple sensory features of the stimulus, since the responses of some neurons could be readily reversed if the meaning of the stimulus (i.e. whether it was food or aversive) was changed, even though its physical appearance remained identical. Further, some bimodal neurons received convergent visual and gustatory inputs, with matching selectivity for the same stimulus in both modalities, again suggesting that an explanation in terms of simple sensory features is inadequate.

Neurons were also studied during the performance of tasks known to be disrupted by orbitofrontal lesions, including a go/no go visual discrimination task and its reversal. 8.6% of neurons had differential responses to the two discriminative stimuli in the task, one of which indicated that reward was available and the other saline. Reversing the meaning of the two stimuli showed that whereas some differential units were closely linked to the sensory features of the stimuli, and some to their behavioural significance, others were conditional, in that they would only respond if a particular stimulus was present, and if it was the one being currently rewarded. Other neurons had activity related to the outcome of the animal's response, with some indicating that reinforcement had been received and others, that an error had been made and that a reversal was required.

Thus, neurons in the orbitofrontal cortex possess highly coded information about which stimuli are present, as well as information about the consequences of the animal's own responses. It is suggested that together they may constitute a neuronal mechanism for determining whether particular visual stimuli continue to be associated with reinforcement, as well as providing for the modification of the animal's behavioural responses to such stimuli when those responses are no longer appropriate.

Key words

Orbitofrontal cortex Reinforcement Feeding Visual discrimination Reversal Frontal lobe 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Benevento LA, Fallon JH, Davis BJ, Rezak M (1977) Auditory-visual interaction in single cells of the superior temporal sulcus and orbito-frontal cortex of the macaque monkey. Exp Neurol 57: 849–872CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Benjamin RM, Jackson JC (1974) Unit discharges in the mediodorsal nucleus of the squirrel monkey evoked by electrical stimulation of the olfactory bulb. Brain Res 75: 181–191Google Scholar
  3. Bos J, Benevento LA (1975) Projections of the medial pulvinar to orbitofrontal cortex and frontal eye fields in the rhesus monkey. Exp Neurol 49: 487–496Google Scholar
  4. Brutkowski S, Mishkin M, Rosvold HE (1963) Positive and inhibitory motor conditioned reflexes in monkeys after ablation of orbital or dorso-lateral surface of the frontal cortex. In: Gutman E, Hnik P (eds) Central and peripheral mechanisms of motor functions. Czechoslovak Academy of Sciences, Prague, pp 133–141Google Scholar
  5. Burton MJ, Rolls ET, Mora F (1976) Effects of hunger on the responses of neurons in the lateral hypothalamus to the sight and taste of food. Exp Neurol 51: 668–677Google Scholar
  6. Butter CM (1969) Perseveration in extinction and in discrimination reversal tasks following selective frontal ablations in Macaca mulatta. Physiol Behav 4: 163–171Google Scholar
  7. Butter CM, Mishkin M, Rosvold HE (1963) Conditioning and extinction of a food-rewarded response after selective ablations of frontal cortex in rhesus monkeys. Exp Neurol 7: 65–75Google Scholar
  8. Butter CM, Mishkin M, Mirsky AF (1968) Emotional responses toward humans in monkeys with selective frontal lesions. Physiol Behav 3: 213–215Google Scholar
  9. Butter CM, McDonald JA, Snyder DA (1969) Orality, preference behavior, and reinforcement value of nonfood objects in monkeys with orbital frontal lesions. Science 164: 1306–1307Google Scholar
  10. Butter CM, Snyder DR, McDonald JA (1970) Effects of orbitofrontal lesions on aversive and aggressive behaviours in rhesus monkeys. J Comp Physiol Psychol 72: 132–144Google Scholar
  11. Butter CM, Snyder DR (1972) Alterations in aversive and aggressive behaviors following orbitofrontal lesions in rhesus monkeys. Acta Neurobiol Exp (Warsz) 32: 525–565Google Scholar
  12. Chavis DA, Pandya DN (1976) Further observations on the corticofrontal connections in rhesus monkey. Brain Res 117: 369–386Google Scholar
  13. Fuster JM (1980) The Prefrontal cortex. Raven Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  14. Goldberg ME, Bushnell MC (1981) Behavioral enhancement of visual responses in monkey cerebral cortex. II. Modulation in frontal eye fields specifically related to saccades. J Neurophysiol 46: 773–787Google Scholar
  15. Iversen SD, Mishkin M (1970) Perseverative interference in monkey following selective lesions of the inferior prefrontal convexity. Exp Brain Res 11: 376–386PubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Jacobsen S, Butters N, Kowalski H (1978) Subcortical projections to the orbital region of the frontal lobe. Soc Neurosci Abstr 4: 222Google Scholar
  17. Jones B, Mishkin M (1972) Limbic lesions and the problem of stimulus-reinforcement associations. Exp Neurol 36: 362–377CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Jones EG (1969) Interrelationships of parieto-temporal and frontal cortex in the rhesus monkey. Brain Res 13: 412–415Google Scholar
  19. Jones EG, Powell TPS (1970) An anatomical study of converging sensory pathways within the cerebral cortex of the monkey. Brain 93: 793–821PubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Kojima S (1980) Prefrontal unit activity in the monkey: Relation to visual stimuli and movements. Exp Neurol 69: 110–123Google Scholar
  21. Kubota K, Iwamoto T, Suzuki H (1974) Visuokinetic activities of primate prefrontal neurons during delayed-response performance. J Neurophysiol 37: 1197–1212Google Scholar
  22. Kuypers HGJM, Szwarcbart MK, Mishkin M, Rosvold HE (1965) Occipitotemporal corticocortical connections in the rhesus monkey. Exp Neurol 11: 245–262Google Scholar
  23. Lawicka W, Mishkin M, Rosvold HE (1966) Dissociation of impairment on auditory tasks following orbital and dorsolateral frontal lesions in monkeys. Proc X Congr Pol Physiol Soc, Warshaw, pp 178–179Google Scholar
  24. Lawicka W, Mishkin M, Rosvold HE (1975) Dissociation of deficits on auditory tasks following partial prefrontal lesions in monkeys. Acta Neurobiol Exp (Warsz) 35: 584–607Google Scholar
  25. Markowitsch HJ, Pritzel M (1976) Reward-related neurons in cat association cortex. Brain Res 111: 185–188Google Scholar
  26. Markowitsch HJ, Pritzel M (1978) Single-unit activity in cat prefrontal and posterior parietal cortex during performance of spatial reversal tasks. Brain Res 149: 53–76Google Scholar
  27. Merrill EG, Ainsworth A (1972) Glass-coated platinum-plated tungsten microelectrodes. Med Biol Eng 10: 662–672Google Scholar
  28. Mikami A, Ito S, Kubota K (1978) Responses of prefrontal neurons to extrafoveal slit stimulation in a visual fixation task of monkeys. J Physiol Soc Japan 40: 269Google Scholar
  29. Milner B (1964) Some effects of frontal lobectomy in man. In: Warren JM, Akert K (eds) The frontal granular cortex and behavior. McGraw-Hill, New York, pp 313–334Google Scholar
  30. Mishkin M (1964) Perseveration of central sets after frontal lesions in monkeys. In: Warren JM, Akert K (eds) The frontal granular cortex and behavior. McGraw Hill, New York, pp 219–241Google Scholar
  31. Mishkin M, Manning FJ (1978) Non-spatial memory after selective prefrontal lesions in monkeys. Brain Res 143: 313–324Google Scholar
  32. Mishkin M, Vest B, Waxler M, Rosvold HE (1969) A reexamination of the effects of frontal lobe lesions on object alternation. Neuropsychologia 7: 357–363Google Scholar
  33. Mohler CW, Goldberg ME, Wurtz RH (1973) Visual receptive fields of frontal eye field neurons. Brain Res 61: 385–389Google Scholar
  34. Mora F, Avrith DB, Phillips AG, Rolls ET (1979) Effects of satiety on self-stimulation of the orbitofrontal cortex in the monkey. Neurosci Lett 13: 141–145Google Scholar
  35. Nauta WJH (1971) The problem of the frontal lobe: A reinterpretation. J Psychiatr Res 8: 167–187Google Scholar
  36. Nauta WJH (1972) Neural associations of the frontal cortex. Acta Neurobiol Exp (Warsz) 32: 125–140Google Scholar
  37. Niki H, Sakai M, Kubota K (1972) Delayed alternation performance and unit activity of the caudate head and medial orbitofrontal gyrus in the monkey. Brain Res 38: 343–353Google Scholar
  38. Niki H, Watanabe M (1979) Prefrontal and cingulate unit activity during timing behavior in the monkey. Brain Res 171: 213–224Google Scholar
  39. Pandya DN, Kuypers H (1969) Cortico-cortical connections in the rhesus monkey. Brain Res 13: 13–36Google Scholar
  40. Pigarev IN, Rizzolatti G, Scandolara C (1979) Neurons responding to visual stimuli in the frontal lobe of macaque monkeys. Neurosci Lett 12: 207–212Google Scholar
  41. Potter H, Nauta WJH (1979) A note on the problem of olfactory associations of the orbitofrontal cortex in the monkey. Neuroscience 4: 361–367Google Scholar
  42. Rizzolatti G, Scandolara C, Matelli M, Gentilluci M (1981) Afferent properties of periarcuate neurons in macaque monkeys. II. Visual responses. Behav Brain Res 2: 147–164CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. Rolls ET (1975) The brain and reward. Pergamon Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  44. Rolls ET (1981) Processing beyond the inferior temporal visual cortex related to feeding, learning, and striatal function. In: Katsuki Y, Norgren R, Sato M (eds) Brain mechanisms of sensation, chapt 16. Wiley, New York, pp 241–269Google Scholar
  45. Rolls ET, Burton MJ, Mora F (1976) Hypothalamic neuronal responses associated with the sight of food. Brain Res 111: 53–66Google Scholar
  46. Rolls ET, Judge SJ, Sanghera MK (1977) Activity of neurons in the inferotemporal cortex of the alert monkey. Brain Res 130: 229–238Google Scholar
  47. Rolls ET, Sanghera MK, Roper-Hall A (1979) The latency of activation of neurons in the lateral hypothalamus and substantia innominata during feeding in the monkey. Brain Res 164: 121–135Google Scholar
  48. Rolls ET, Burton MJ, Mora F (1980) Neurophysiological analysis of brain-stimulation reward in the monkey. Brain Res 194: 339–357Google Scholar
  49. Rosenkilde CE, Bauer RH, Fuster JM (1981) Single-unit activity in ventral prefrontal cortex of behaving monkeys. Brain Res 209: 375–394Google Scholar
  50. Sanghera MK, Rolls ET, Roper-Hall A (1979) Visual responses of neurons in the dorsolateral amygdala of the alert monkey. Exp Neurol 63: 610–626Google Scholar
  51. Suzuki H, Azuma M (1977) Prefrontal unit activity during gazing at a light spot in the monkey. Brain Res 126: 497–508Google Scholar
  52. Suzuki H, Azuma M, Yumiya H (1979) Stimulus and behavioural factors contributing to the activation of monkey prefrontal neurons during gazing. Jpn J Physiol 29: 471–490Google Scholar
  53. Tanabe T, Yarita H, Iino M, Ooshima Y, Takagi SR (1975) An olfactory projection area in the orbitofrontal cortex of the monkey. J Neurophysiol 38: 1269–1283Google Scholar
  54. Tanaka D (1973) Effects of selective prefrontal decortication on escape behavior in the monkey. Brain Res 53: 161–173Google Scholar
  55. Thorpe SJ, Maddison S, Rolls ET (1979) Single unit activity in the orbitofrontal cortex of the behaving animal. Neurosci Lett 3: S77Google Scholar
  56. Ursin H, Rosvold HE, Vest B (1969) Food preference in brain lesioned monkeys. Physiol Behav 4: 609–612Google Scholar
  57. Watanabe M (1982) Prefrontal unit activity during delayed conditional discriminations in the monkey. Brain Res 225: 51–66Google Scholar
  58. Woodward RR, Goldsmith PL (1964) Cumulative sum techniques. Mathematical and statistical techniques for industry. ICI Monograph, no. 3. Oliver and Boyd, EdinburghGoogle Scholar
  59. Wurtz RH, Mohler CW (1976) Enhancement of visual responses in monkey striate cortex and frontal eye fields. J Neurophysiol 39: 766–772Google Scholar
  60. Yarita H, Iino M, Tanabe T, Kogure S, Takagi SF (1980) A transthalamic olfactory pathway to orbitofrontal cortex in the monkey. J Neurophysiol 43: 69–85Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 1983

Authors and Affiliations

  • S. J. Thorpe
    • 1
  • E. T. Rolls
    • 1
  • S. Maddison
    • 1
  1. 1.Dept. of Experimental PsychologyUniversity of OxfordOxfordEngland

Personalised recommendations