Skip to main content
Log in

Representing three-dimensional objects by sets of activities of receptive fields

  • Published:
Biological Cybernetics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Idealized models of receptive fields (RFs) can be used as building blocks for the creation of powerful distributed computation systems. The present report concentrates on investigating the utility of collections of RFs in representing three-dimensional objects under changing viewing conditions. The main requirement in this task is that the pattern of activity of RFs vary as little as possible when the object and the camera move relative to each other. I propose a method for representing objects by RF activities, based on the observation that, in the case of rotation around a fixed axis, differences of activities of RFs that are properly situated with respect to that axis remain invariant. Results of computational experiments suggest that a representation scheme based on this algorithm for the choice of stable pairs of RFs would perform consistently better than a scheme involving random sets of RFs. The proposed scheme may be useful under object or camera rotation, both for ideal lambertian objects, and for real-world objects such as human faces.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Aloimonos JY, Shulman D (1989) Integration of visual modules: an extension of the Marr paradigm. Academic Press, Boston

    Google Scholar 

  • Aloimonos JY, Weiss I, Bandopadhay A (1988) Active vision. Int J Comput Vision 2:333–356

    Google Scholar 

  • Amari S (1968) Invariant structures of signal and feature spaces in pattern recognition problems. RAAG Mem 4:553–566

    Google Scholar 

  • Amari S (1978) Feature spaces which admit and detect invariant signal transformations. In: Proc 4th Intl Conf on Pattern Recognition, Tokyo, pp 452–456

  • Amari S, Maruyama M (1987) A theory on the determination of 3D motion and 3D structure from features. Spatial Vision 2:151–168

    Google Scholar 

  • Bishop PO, Coombs JS, Henry GH (1973) Receptive fields of simple cells in the cat striate cortex. J Physiol (Lond) 231:31–60

    Google Scholar 

  • Blinn JF (1988), Models of light reflection for computer-synthesized pictures. In: Richards W (eds) Natural computation. MIT Press, Cambridge, Mass, pp 214–223

    Google Scholar 

  • Bülthoff HH, Edelman S (1992) Psychophysical support for a 2-D view interpolation theory of object recognition. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 89:60–64

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawis S, Shapley R, Kaplan E, Tranchina D (1984) The receptive field organization of X-cells in the cat: spatiotemporal coupling and asymmetry. Vision Res 24:549–564

    Google Scholar 

  • Edelman S (1992) Class similarity and viewpoint invariance in the recognition of 3D objects. CS-TR 92-17, Weizmann Institute of Science

  • Edelman S, Reisfeld D, Yeshurun Y (1992) Learning to recognize faces from examples. In Sandini G (eds) Proceedings of the 2nd European Conference on Computer Vision. (Lecture notes in computer science, vol 588) Springer, Berlin Heidelberg New York, pp 787–791

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris CS (1980) Insight or out of sight? Two examples of perceptual plasticity in the human adult. In: Harris CS (eds) Visual Coding and Adaptability. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp 95–149

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris CS, Gibson AR (1968) Is orientation-specific color adaptation in human vision due to edge detectors, afterimages, or “dipoles”? Science 162:1506–1507

    Google Scholar 

  • Hubel DH, Wiesel TN (1959) Receptive fields of single neurons in the cat's striate cortex. J Physiol (Lond) 148:574–591

    Google Scholar 

  • Hurlbert A, Poggio T (1988) Synthesizing a color algorithm from examples. Science 239:482–485

    Google Scholar 

  • Intrator N (1992) Feature extraction using an unsupervised neural network. Neural Computation 4:98–107

    Google Scholar 

  • Intrator N, Cooper LN (1992) Objective function formulation of the BCM theory of visual cortical plasticity: statistical connections, stability conditions. Neural Networks 5:3–17

    Google Scholar 

  • Kanatani K (1990) Group-theoretical methods in image understanding. Springer, Berlin Heidelberg New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Katz LC, Callaway EM (1992) Development of local circuits in mammalian visual cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci 15:31–56

    Google Scholar 

  • Koenderink JJ, Doorn AJ van (1986) Optic flow. Vision Res 26:161–180

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuffler SW (1953) Discharge patterns and functional organization of mammalian retina. J Neurophysiol 16:37–68

    Google Scholar 

  • Malach R, Amir Y, Bartfeld E, Grinvald A (1992) Biocytin injections, guided by optical imaging, reveal relationships between functional architecture and intrinsic connections in monkey visual cortex. Soc Neurosci Abstr 18:364

    Google Scholar 

  • Mallot HA, Seelen W von, Giannakopoulos F (1990) Neural mapping and space-variant image processing. Neural Networks 3:16–25

    Google Scholar 

  • McCollough C (1965) Color adaptation of edge detectors in the human visual system. Science 149:1115–1116

    Google Scholar 

  • Moses Y, Edelman S, Ullman S (1993) Generalization across illumination and orientation changes in inverted and upright faces. CS-TR 93-14, Weizmann Institute of Science

  • Mundy JL, Zisserman A (eds) (1992) Geometric invariance in computer vision. MIT Press, Cambridge, Mass

    Google Scholar 

  • Nishihara HK, Poggio T (1984) Stereo vision for robotics. In: Brady JM, Paul R (eds) Robotics research: the first international symposium. MIT Press, Cambridge, Mass, pp 489–505

    Google Scholar 

  • Poggio T (1990) A theory of how the brain might work. Cold Spring Harb Symp Quant Biol 55:899–910

    Google Scholar 

  • Poggio T, Edelman S (1990) A network that learns to recognize three-dimensional objects. Nature 343:263–266

    Google Scholar 

  • Poggio T, Girosi F (1990) Regularization algorithms for learning that are equivalent to multilayer networks. Science 247:978–982

    Google Scholar 

  • Poggio T, Fahle M Edelman S (1992) Fast perceptual learning in visual hyperacuity. Science 256:1018–1021

    Google Scholar 

  • Poggio T, Yang W, Torre V (1989) Optical flow: computational properties and net-works, biological and analog. In: Durbin R, Miall C, Mitchison G (eds) The computing neuron. Addison-Wesley, New York, pp 355–370

    Google Scholar 

  • Polat U, Sagi D (1992) Lateral interactions between spatial filters: excitation and inhibition affected by spatial configuration. Perception 21 [Suppl 2]:92

    Google Scholar 

  • Snippe HP, Koenderink JJ (1992) Discrimination thresholds for channel-coded systems. Biol Cybern 66:543–551

    Google Scholar 

  • Spitzer H, Hochstein S (1988) Complex-cell receptive field models. Prog Neurobiol 31:285–309

    Google Scholar 

  • Torrance KE, Sparrow EM (1966) Polarization, directional distribution, and off-specular peak phenomena in light reflected from roughened surfaces. J Opt Soc Am 56:916–925

    Google Scholar 

  • Ullman S (1979) The interpretation of visual motion. MIT Press, Cambridge, Mass

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Edelman, S. Representing three-dimensional objects by sets of activities of receptive fields. Biol. Cybern. 70, 37–45 (1993). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00202564

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00202564

Keywords

Navigation