Advertisement

Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology

, Volume 33, Issue 2, pp 67–72 | Cite as

Acoustical signals in the dance language of the giant honeybee, Apis dorsata

  • Wolfgang H. Kirchner
  • Claudia Dreller
Article

Summary

Acoustical signals emitted by dancing bees have recently been shown to transmit information about the location of food sources in the western honeybee, Apis mellifera. Towne (1985) reported that in the Asian honeybee species Apis dorsata, which builds a single comb in the open under overhanging rocks or tree branches, sound signals were not emitted by the dancers. This led to the conclusion that acoustical communication is restricted to bees that nest in the dark, like A. mellifera. Here we show that in fact A. dorsata produces dance sounds similar to those emitted by A. mellifera, and that these acoustical signals contain information about distance, direction and profitability of food sources. The acoustical transfer of information has thus evolved independently of nesting in dark cavities. The significance of nocturnal activity in Apis dorsata for the evolution of sound communication is discussed.

Key words

Communication Sound Asian honey bees Apsis dorsata 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Abrol DP, Kapil RP (1989) Morphometrics and wing stroke frequency of some bees. Proc Indian Natl Sci Acad B55:369–376Google Scholar
  2. Dyer FC (1985a) Mechanisms of dance orientation in the Asian honey bee Apis florea L. J Comp Physiol A 157:183–198Google Scholar
  3. Dyer FC (1985b) Nocturnal orientation by the Asian honey bee, Apis dorsata. Anim Behav 33:769–774Google Scholar
  4. Dyer FC, Seeley TD (1987) Interspecific comparison of endothermy in honeybees (Apis): deviations from the expected size-related pattern. J Exp Biol 127:1–26Google Scholar
  5. Dyer FC, Seeley TD (1991) Dance dialects and foraging range in three Asian honey bee species. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 28:227–233Google Scholar
  6. Esch H (1961) Über die Schallerzeugung beim Werbetanz der Honigbiene. Z Vergl Physiol 45:1–11Google Scholar
  7. Esch H (1964) Beiträge zum Problem der Entfernungsweisung in den Schwänzeltänzen der Honigbiene. Z Vergl Physiol 48:534–546Google Scholar
  8. Fletcher DJC (1978) The African bee, Apis mellifera adansonii, in Africa. Annu Rev Entomol 23:151–171CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Frisch K von (1923) Über die “Sprache” der Bienen, eine tierpsychologische Untersuchung. Zool Jb (Physiol) 40:1–186Google Scholar
  10. Frisch K von (1965) Tanzsprache and Orientierung der Bienen. Springer, Berlin Heidelberg New YorkGoogle Scholar
  11. Gould JL, Dyer FC, Towne WF (1985) Recent study of the dance language. In: Hölldobler B, Lindauer M (eds) Experimental behavioral ecology and sociobiology (Fortschritte der Zoologie, Bd. 31). Fischer, Stuttgart New York, pp 141–161Google Scholar
  12. Kerfoot WB (1967) The lunar periodicity of Sphecodogastra texana, a nocturnal bee (Hymmenoptera: Halictidae). Anim Behav 15:479–486Google Scholar
  13. Kirchner WH, Sommer K (1992) The dance language of the honeybee mutant diminutive wings. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 30:181–184Google Scholar
  14. Kirchner WH, Lindauer M, Michelsen A (1988) Honeybee dance communication: Acoustical indication of direction in round dances. Naturwissenschaften 75:629–630Google Scholar
  15. Kirchner WH, Dreller C, Towne WF (1991) Hearing in honeybees: operant conditioning and spontaneous reactions to airborne sound. J Comp Physiol A 168:85–89Google Scholar
  16. Lindauer M (1956) Über die Verständigung bei den indischen Bienen. Z Vergl Physiol 38:521–557Google Scholar
  17. Lindauer M (1975) Verstandigung im Bienenstaat. Fischer Taschenbuch, StuttgartGoogle Scholar
  18. Michelsen A, Kirchner WH, Lindauer M (1986) Sound and vibrational signals in the dance language of the honeybee, Apis mellifera. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 18:207–212Google Scholar
  19. Michelsen A, Towne WF, Kirchner WH, Kryger P (1987) The acoustic near field of a dancing honeybee. J Comp Physiol A 161:633–643Google Scholar
  20. Michelsen A, Andersen BB, Kirchner WH, Lindauer M (1989) Honeybees can be recruited by a mechanical model of a dancing bee. Naturwissenschaften 76:277–280Google Scholar
  21. Michelsen A, Andersen BB, Storm J, Kirchner WH, Lindauer M (1992) How honeybees perceive communication dances, studied by means of a mechanical model. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 30:143–150Google Scholar
  22. Punchihewa RWK, Koeniger N, Kevan PG, Gadawski RM (1985) Observations on the dance communication and natural foraging ranges ranges of Apis cerana, A. dorsata and A. florea in Sri Lanka. J Apic Res 24:168–175Google Scholar
  23. Roubik DW (1989) Ecology; and natural history of tropical bees. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  24. Spangler HG (1991) Do honeybees encode distance information into wing vibrations of the waggle dance? J Insect Behav 4:15–20Google Scholar
  25. Smith DR (ed) (1991) Diversity in the ganus Apis. Westview Press, Boulder San Francisco OxfordGoogle Scholar
  26. Towne WF (1985) Acoustical and visual cues in the dances of four honey bee species. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 16:185–187PubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Towne WF, Kirchner WH (1989) Hearing in honey bees: Detection of air-particle oscillations. Science 244:686–688Google Scholar
  28. Waddington KD, Kirchner WH (1992) Acoustical and behavioral correlates of profitability of food sources in honey bee round dances. Ethology 92:1–6Google Scholar
  29. Wenner AM (1962) Sound production during the waggle dance of the honey bee. Anim Behav 10: 79–95Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 1993

Authors and Affiliations

  • Wolfgang H. Kirchner
    • 1
  • Claudia Dreller
    • 1
  1. 1.Theodor-Boveri-Institut für Biowissenschaften der UniversitätLehrstuhl für Verhaltensphysiologie und SoziobiologieWürzburgGermany

Personalised recommendations