Advertisement

Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology

, Volume 32, Issue 2, pp 111–118 | Cite as

Brown pelican siblicide and the prey-size hypothesis

  • D. Pinson
  • H. Drummond
Article

Summary

We asked whether the brown pelican (Pelecanus occidentalis) practices facultative brood reduction and tested two predictions of Mock's (1985) prey-size hypothesis: (1) if chicks take food directly from the parental mouth, nestmates should compete aggressively; (2) aggression between nestmates should increase during the developmental transition from indirect feeding (parents deposit food on the substrate) to direct feeding (parents pass food from mouth to mouth). Eggs in two-egg and three-egg clutches were laid and hatched 2 days apart. Junior (second- and third-hatched) chicks, fed less than their nestmates, grew more slowly and died more frequently. Of non-hatchling victims 74% were underweight, 55% bore peck wounds and 43% were found outside the nest, implying starvation, aggression and expulsion from the nest. Aggression occurred in all observed sibships, and junior chicks were subordinate in eight of ten cases. A progressive transition from indirect to direct feeding occurred during the 30 days after hatching, but there was no evidence of an increase in pecking during the transition. In conclusion, the brown pelican practices facultative brood reduction, and chick loss is mediated by sibling aggression as predicted by the preysize hypothesis. Pecking during indirect feeds seems to contradict the prey-size hypothesis, but may serve partly to establish a relationship of dominance-subordinance.

Keywords

Developmental Transition Brood Reduction Direct Feeding Progressive Transition Deposit Food 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Anderson DW, Gress F, Mais KF (1982) Brown pelicans: influence of food suply on reproduction. Oikos 39:23–31Google Scholar
  2. Blus LJ, Keahey JA (1978) Variation in reproductfty with age in the brown pelican. Auk 95:128–134Google Scholar
  3. Burke VEM, Brown LH (1970) Observations on the breeding of the pink-backed pelican. Ibis 112:499–512Google Scholar
  4. Cash KJ, Evans RM (1986) Brood reduction in the American white pelican (Pelecanus erythrorhynchos). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 18:413–418Google Scholar
  5. Chapman FM (1908) Camps and cruises of an ornithologist. Appleton, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  6. Clark AB, Wilson DS (1981) Avian breeding adaptations: hatching asynchrony, brood reduction, and nest failure. Q Rev Biol 56:253–277Google Scholar
  7. Cooper J (1980) Fatal sibling aggression in pelicans — a review. Ostrich 51:183–186Google Scholar
  8. Drummond H (1987) A review of parent-offspring conflict and brood reduction in the Pelecaniformes. Colon Waterbirds 10:1–15Google Scholar
  9. Drummond H, Osorno JL (in press) Training siblings to be submissive losers: dominance between booby nestlings. Anim BehavGoogle Scholar
  10. Drummond H, Gonzalez E, Osorno JL (1986) Parent-offspring cooperation in the blue-footed booby (Sula nebouxii). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 19:365–372Google Scholar
  11. Drummond H, Osorno JL, Garcia C, Torres R, Merchant H (1991) Sexual dimorphism, and sibling competition: implications for avian sex ratios. Am Nat 138:623–641Google Scholar
  12. Lack D (1947) The significance of clutch size. Ibis 89:302–352Google Scholar
  13. Lack D (1954) The natural regulation of animal numbers. Clarendon, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  14. Magrath RG (1989) Hatching asynchrony and reproductive success in the blackbird. Nature 339:536–538Google Scholar
  15. Mock DW (1984a) Infanticide, siblicide and avian nestling mortality. In: Hausfater G, Hrdy SB (eds) Infanticide: comparative and evolutionary perspectives. Academic, New York, pp 3–30Google Scholar
  16. Mock DW (1984b) Siblicidal aggression and resource monopolization in birds. Science 225:731–733Google Scholar
  17. Mock DW (1985) Siblicidal brood reduction: the prey size hypothesis. Am Nat 125:327–343Google Scholar
  18. Mock DW, Parker GA (1986) Advantages and disadvantages of egret and heron brood reduction. Evolution 40:459–470Google Scholar
  19. Mock DW, Lamey TC, Williams CF, Pelletier A (1987) Flexibility in the development of heron sibling aggression: an intraspecific test of the prey-size hypothesis. Anim Behav 35:1386–1393Google Scholar
  20. Mock DW, Drummond H, Stinson CH (1990) Avian siblicide. Am Sci 78:438–449Google Scholar
  21. Schreiber RW (1976) Growth and development of nestling brown pelicans. Bird Banding 47:19–39Google Scholar
  22. Schreiber RW (1979) Reproductive performance of the eastern brown pelican. Contrib Sci Nat Hist Mus Los Angeles County 317:1–43Google Scholar
  23. Schreiber RW, Risebrough RW (1972) Studies of the brown pelican. Wilson Bull 84:119–135Google Scholar
  24. Vesey-Fitzgerald D (1957) The breeding of the white pelican Pelecanus onocrotalus in the Rukwa Valley Tanganyika. Bull Br Ornithol Club 77:127–129Google Scholar
  25. Vestjens JWM (1977) Breeding behaviour of the Australian pelican, Pelecanus conspicillatus in New South Wales. Aust Wildl Res 4:37–58Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 1993

Authors and Affiliations

  • D. Pinson
    • 1
  • H. Drummond
    • 1
  1. 1.Centro de EcologiaUniversidad Nacional Autónoma de MéxicoMexico

Personalised recommendations