Skip to main content
Log in

On some subject/object non-asymmetries in Mohawk

  • Published:
Natural Language & Linguistic Theory Aims and scope Submit manuscript

This article considers certain differences between subjects and objects in English that are not found in Mohawk, a nonconfigurational language with extensive agreement morphology. In particular, disjoint reference effects, island conditions, and weak crossover phenomena are investigated in some detail. Facts from these domains motivate a theory of Mohawk clause structure in which most NPs are generated in adjunct positions, along the lines proposed by Jelinek (1984) and others. Clausal arguments, however, do show standard subject-object asymmetries, unlike NPs. This motivates a Case-driven theory of nonconfigurationality, and shows that it is correct to attribute configurational representations to Mohawk after all.

The data presented in this article is a representative subset of the data collected by the author on these topics; fuller paradigms are available upon written request.

Mohawk examples are given in the practical orthography described in Deering and Delisle (1976), with the following four changes: (i) the mid unround nasal vowel is written [v] instead of [en]; (ii) the back round nasal vowel is written [u] instead of [on]; (iii) [y] is distinguished from [i]; (iv) stress and vowel length are not marked, these being predictable. Complex sound changes often happen at morpheme boundaries in Mohawk; in some cases the forms given are closer to underlying representations, in other cases they are closer to surface representations.

The following abbreviations are used in the glosses: fact, factual mode; fut, future mode; punc, punctual aspect; hab, habitual aspect; rev, reversive; stat, stative aspect; srfl, semireflexive; dup, duplicative; cis, cislocative; trans, translocative; sim, simultaneous; opt, optative; part, partitive; neg, negative; Q, question particle; iter, iterative. Glosses of agreement include indication of person/gender (1, 2, M, F, N), number (s, d, or p), and series (S (roughly subject), 0 (roughly object), or P (possessor)).

The research reported here was generously supported by The Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada, grants #410-89-0207 and #410-90-0308, and by FCAR of Quebec, grant #91 -ER-0578. It has been presented to audiences at MIT, Northwestern University, University of Illinois, Princeton University, University of Maryland, University of Stuttgart, University of Geneva, and the Montreal Linguistics Circle. In addition to these audiences, I also wish to thank Jose Bonneau, David Pesetsky, Lisa Travis, Dan Everett, Adriana Chamorro, Edward Ikeda, Ken Hale, Eloise Jelinek, Peggy Speas, Juan Uriagereka, and Paul Postal (as NLLT reviewer) for their comments, suggestions, and help in various ways.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Baker, Mark: 1988, Incorporation: A Theory of Grammatical Function Changing, University of Chicago Press, Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baker, Mark: 1990a, ‘Pronominal Inflection and the Morphology-Syntax Interface’, in Proceedings of the the 26th annual meeting of the Chicago Linguistics Society.

  • Baker, Mark: 1990b, ‘On the Absence of Certain Quantifiers in Mohawk’, unpublished, McGill University.

  • Baker, Mark: In progress, ‘Comitatives, Binding, and Plural Pronouns’, unpublished, McGill University.

  • Benger, Janet: 1990, ‘Morphologically Bound Pronominals in Mohawk: Configurationality Within the Verb Complex’, unpublished, University of Ottawa.

  • Borer, Hagit: 1984, Parametric Syntax: Case Studies in Semitic and Romance Languages, Foris, Dordrecht.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bresnan, Joan: 1982, ‘Control and Complementation’, in J. Bresnan (ed.), The Mental Representation of Grammatical Relations, MIT Press, Cambridge, Mass., pp. 282–390.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, Noam: 1973, ‘Conditions on Transformations’, in S. Anderson, and P. Kiparsky (eds.), A Festschrift for Morris Halle, Holt, Rinehart and Winston, New York, pp. 232–286.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, Noam: 1981, Lectures on Government and Binding, Foris, Dordrecht.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, Noam: 1982, Some Concepts and Consequences of the Theory of Government and Binding, MIT Press, Cambridge, Mass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, Noam: 1986a, Barriers, MIT Press, Cambridge, Mass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, Noam: 1986b, Knowledge of Language: Its Nature, Origin, and Use, Praeger, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cinque, Guglielmo: 1990, Types of A'-Dependencies, MIT Press, Cambridge, Mass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deering, Nora & Helga Harris Delisle: 1976, Mohawk: A Teaching Grammar, Thunderbird Press, Kahnawake, Quebec.

    Google Scholar 

  • Farmer, Ann: 1984. Modularity in Syntax, MIT Press, Cambridge, Mass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fukui, Naoki, and Margaret Speas: 1986, ‘Specifiers and Projections’, MIT Working Papers in Linguistics 8, 128–172.

    Google Scholar 

  • Giorgi, Alessandra, and Giuseppe Longobardi: to appear, The Syntax of Noun Phrases: Configuration, Parameters and Empty Categories, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.

  • Hale, Kenneth: 1983, ‘Warlpiri and the Grammar of Nonconfigurational Languages’, Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 1, 5–49.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huang, C.-T. James: 1982, Logical Relations in Chinese and the Theory of Grammar, unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, MIT.

  • Jelinek, Eloise: 1984, ‘Empty Categories, Case, and Configurationality’, Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 2, 39–76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jelinek, Eloise: 1988, ‘The Case Split and Pronominal Arguments in Choctaw’, in L. Marácz and P. Muysken (eds.), Configurationality: The Typology of Asymmetries, Foris, Dordrecht.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jelinek, Eloise: 1989, ‘Argument Type in Athabaskan: Evidence From Noun Incorporation’, unpublished, University of Arizona.

  • Kayne, Richard: 1984, Connectedness and Binary Branching, Foris, Dordrecht.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kitagawa, Yoshihisa: 1986, Subjects in Japanese and English, unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, University of Massachusetts, Amherst.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koopman, Hilda and Dominique Sportiche: 1988, ‘Subjects’, unpublished, UCLA.

  • Kuroda, S. Y.: 1988, ‘Whether We Agree or Not’, in W. Poser (ed.), Papers From the Second International Workshop on Japanese Syntax, CSLI, Stanford, pp. 103–143.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lasnik, Howard: 1989, Essays on Anaphora, Kluwer, Dordrecht.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lounsbury, Floyd: 1953, Oneida Verb Morphology, Yale University Press, New Haven, Conn.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mithun, Marianne: 1986, ‘When Zero Isn't There’, Proceedings of the 12th Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society, Berkeley, pp. 195–211.

  • Mithun, Marianne: 1987, ‘Is Basic Word Order Universal?’ in R. Tomlin (ed.), Coherence and Grounding in Discourse, Benjamins, Amsterdam, pp. 281–328.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mithun, Marianne, and Wallace Chafe: 1979, ‘Recapturing the Mohawk Language’, in T. Shopen (ed.), Languages and Their Status, Winthrop, Cambridge, Mass., pp. 3–34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mohanan, K. P.: 1982, ‘Grammatical Relations and Clause Structure in Malayalam’, in J. Bresnan (ed.), The Mental Representation of Grammatical Relations, MIT Press, Cambridge, Mass., pp. 504–589.

    Google Scholar 

  • Postal, Paul: 1979, Some Syntactic Rules in Mohawk, Garland, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raposo, Eduardo and Juan Uriagereka: 1990, ‘Long-Distance Case Assignment’, Linguistic Inquiry 21, 505–538.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reinhart, Tanya: 1976, The Syntactic Domain of Anaphora, Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, MIT.

  • Reinhart, Tanya: 1983, Anaphora and Semantic Interpretation, University of Chicago Press, Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rizzi, Luigi: 1986, ‘On the Status of Subject Clitics in Romance’, in O. Jaeggli and C. Silva-Corvalan (eds.), Studies in Romance Linguistics, Foris, Dordrecht.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ross, John: 1967, Constraints on Variables in Syntax, unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, MIT.

  • Rouveret, Alain, and Jean-Roger Vergnaud: 1980, ‘Specifying Reference to the Subject’, Linguistic Inquiry 11, 97–202.

    Google Scholar 

  • Safir, Kenneth: 1984, ‘Multiple Variable Binding’, Linguistic Inquiry 15, 603–638.

    Google Scholar 

  • Safir, Kenneth: 1985, Syntactic Chains, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shlonsky, Ur: 1987, Null and Displaced Subjects, unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, MIT.

  • Speas, Margaret: 1990, Phrase Structure in Natural Language, Kluwer, Dordrecht.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tellier, Christine: 1988, Universal Licensing: Implications for Parasitic Gap Constructions, unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, McGill University.

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

Mohawk is a Northern Iroquoian language spoken in Quebec, Ontario, and New York. General information about the language can be found in Postal (1979) and Deering and Delisle (1976). The principal information for this work was Ms. Grace Curotte of Kahnawake, Quebec: I thank her for her patience, hospitality and insight. Each type of sentence was also checked with Ms. Carolee Jacobs of Kahnawake. Additional judgments and information were provided by Mr. Frank Jacobs of Kahnawake, Ms. Margaret Lazore, and Ms. Dorothy Lazore of St. Regis, Quebec.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Baker, M.C. On some subject/object non-asymmetries in Mohawk. Nat Lang Linguist Theory 9, 537–576 (1991). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00134750

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00134750

Keywords

Navigation