Advertisement

Journal of Cultural Cognitive Science

, Volume 3, Supplement 1, pp 89–104 | Cite as

The production of passives by English-Norwegian and Turkish-Norwegian bilinguals: a preliminary investigation using a cross-linguistic structural priming manipulation

  • Gözde MercanEmail author
  • Hanne Gram Simonsen
Research Paper

Abstract

This preliminary study tested cross-linguistic structural priming of passives in two groups of adult bilinguals speaking Norwegian-English and Norwegian-Turkish to understand the nature of syntactic mental representations in bilingual minds. Passives in spoken Norwegian are structurally similar to those in English, whereas passives in Turkish are distinct in being morphologically constructed. A shared syntax account based on the similarity of the structures would predict structural priming effects for the Norwegian-English group but not the Norwegian-Turkish group. Results of a computerized picture description task showed that participants provided more active descriptions than passives, but did not indicate any priming in terms of an increased likelihood of repeating the type of the prime they heard in Norwegian in their target picture description in English or Turkish. This can be related to the small sample size or the observation that there were too few passive productions overall. We thus ran an additional analysis which indicated that participants were more likely to produce passives when the target pictures contained inanimate agents. We also ran a separate analysis on Turkish target responses, which did not reveal an effect of prime type on the emphasis of thematic role (agent vs. patient). However, this analysis also showed that pictures with inanimate agents were more likely to be described with target structures emphasizing the patient rather than the agent. We suggest that further research is required to get a clearer picture of the role of the similarity of passives in cross-linguistic structural priming.

Keywords

Cross-linguistic structural priming Bilingualism Norwegian Turkish English Passives Syntactic representation 

Notes

Acknowledgements

This study was funded by the Scientific and Technological Research Council of Turkey (TÜBİTAK) under the BİDEB 2219 International Postdoctoral Research Fellowship granted for the first author’s postdoctoral research at the Center for Multilingualism in Society across the Lifespan (MultiLing), at the University of Oslo (UiO) (01.05.2014–01.08.2015). The study was conducted on the premises of MultiLing, at UiO, a research center financed by the Research Council of Norway, using the facilities of the center and was thus supported in part by the Research Council of Norway through its Centers of Excellence funding scheme, project number 223265. We thank MultiLing Theme 1 for a grant for language proofreading services for this paper, as well. We would also like to thank Bernt Brendemoen, Emel Türker, Yeşim Sevinç-Brohet and our colleagues at MultiLing for their helpful comments; Murat Mercan for his assistance with Turkish data coding; Danielle Stephan for language proofreading; and Martin J. Pickering, Mirta Vernice, Robert J. Hartsuiker, and Sarah Bernolet for sharing their stimulus pictures. We are grateful to all the participants who volunteered to take part in this study. Finally, we would like to thank the editors of this special issue and the two anonymous reviewers for their insightful comments.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

On behalf of both authors, the corresponding author states that there is no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Åfarli, T. A. (1989). The syntax of Norwegian passive constructions (Vol. 9). Trondheim: University of Trondheim Working Papers in Linguistics.Google Scholar
  2. Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language,59(4), 390–412.Google Scholar
  3. Bada, E., & Bedir, H. (1999). Türkçe’de edilgen çatının kullanım sıklık ve alanları: normal ve kişsiz edilgenlik. In Y. Aksan & M. Aksan (Eds.), Dilbilim Kurultayı Bildirileri (Vol. XII, pp. 141–148). Mersin: Mersin Üniversitesi Yayınları. [Publication in Turkish].Google Scholar
  4. Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language,68(3), 255–278.Google Scholar
  5. Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software,67(1), 1–48.Google Scholar
  6. Bayram, F., Rothman, J., Iverson, M., Kupisch, T., Miller, D., Puig-Mayenco, E., et al. (2017). Differences in use without deficiencies in competence: passives in the Turkish and German of Turkish heritage speakers in Germany. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism.  https://doi.org/10.1080/13670050.2017.1324403.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Bernolet, S., Hartsuiker, R. J., & Pickering, M. J. (2007). Shared syntactic representations in bilinguals: Evidence for the role of word-order repetition. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition,33(5), 931–949.Google Scholar
  8. Bernolet, S., Hartsuiker, R. J., & Pickering, M. J. (2009). Persistence of emphasis in language production: A cross-linguistic approach. Cognition,112(2), 300–317.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Bernolet, S., Hartsuiker, R. J., & Pickering, M. J. (2013). From language-specific to shared syntactic representations: The influence of second language proficiency on syntactic sharing in bilinguals. Cognition,127(3), 287–306.Google Scholar
  10. Biber, D., Johansson, S., Leech, G., Conrad, S., & Finegan, E. (1999). Longman grammar of spoken and written English. London: Longman.Google Scholar
  11. Bock, J. K. (1986). Syntactic persistence in language production. Cognitive Psychology,18, 355–387.Google Scholar
  12. Bock, J. K., Loebell, H., & Morey, R. (1992). From conceptual roles to structural relations: Bridging the syntactic cleft. Psychological Review,99, 150–171.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  13. Branigan, H. P., & Pickering, M. J. (2017). An experimental approach to linguistic representation. Behavioral and Brain Sciences,40, e282.Google Scholar
  14. Branigan, H. P., Pickering, M. J., & Cleland, A. A. (2000). Syntactic coordination in dialogue. Cognition,75, B13–B25.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  15. Branigan, H. P., Pickering, M. J., & Tanaka, M. (2008). Contributions of animacy to grammatical function assignment and word order during production. Lingua,118, 172–189.Google Scholar
  16. Chen, B., Jia, Y., Wang, Z., Dunlap, S., & Shin, J.-A. (2013). Is word-order similarity necessary for cross-linguistic structural priming? Second Language Research,29, 375–389.Google Scholar
  17. Clark, H. H. (1965). Some structural properties of simple active and passive Sentences. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior,4, 370–375.Google Scholar
  18. Csató, É. Á., & Johanson, L. (1998). Turkish. In L. Johanson & É. Á. Csató (Eds.), The Turkic languages (pp. 203–235). London & NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
  19. Dell, G. S., & Ferreira, V. S. (2016). New approaches to structural priming [special issue]. Journal of Memory and Language,91, 1–218.Google Scholar
  20. Desmet, T., & Declercq, M. (2006). Cross-linguistic priming of syntactic hierarchical configuration information. Journal of Memory and Language,54, 610–632.Google Scholar
  21. Engdahl, E. (1999). The choice between bli-passive and s-passive in Danish, Norwegian and Swedish. NORDSEM Report 3, University of Gothenburg.Google Scholar
  22. Erguvanlı, E. E. (1984). The function of word order in Turkish grammar. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
  23. Fleischer, Z., Pickering, M. J., & McLean, J. F. (2012). Shared information structure: Evidence from cross-linguistic priming. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition,15, 568–579.Google Scholar
  24. Givón, T. (1984). Syntax: A functional-typological introduction I. Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
  25. Göksel, A., & Kerslake, C. (2005). Turkish: A comprehensive grammar. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
  26. Göksel, A., & Kerslake, C. (2011). Turkish: An essential grammar. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
  27. Gülşeker Solak, H. (2007). Animacy effect on sentence structure choice: A study on Turkish learners of L2 English. Master’s Thesis, Middle East Technical University, Ankara, Turkey.Google Scholar
  28. Hartsuiker, R. J., Beerts, S., Loncke, M., Desmet, T., & Bernolet, S. (2016). Cross-linguistic structural priming in multilinguals: Further evidence for shared syntax. Journal of Memory and Language,90, 14–30.Google Scholar
  29. Hartsuiker, R. J., & Bernolet, S. (2017). The development of shared syntax in second language learning. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition,20(2), 219–234.Google Scholar
  30. Hartsuiker, R. J., Pickering, M. J., & Veltkamp, E. (2004). Is syntax separate or shared between languages? Cross-linguistic syntactic priming in Spanish-English bilinguals. Psychological Science,15(6), 409–414.Google Scholar
  31. Heydel, M., & Murray, W. S. (2000). Conceptual effects in sentence priming: A cross-linguistic perspective. In M. De Vincenzi & V. Lombardo (Eds.), Cross-linguistic perspectives on language processing (pp. 227–254). Dordrecht: Kluwer.Google Scholar
  32. İşsever, S. (2003). Information structure in Turkish: the word order-prosody interface. Lingua,113, 1025–1053.Google Scholar
  33. Jaeger, T. F. (2008). Categorical data analysis: Away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards logit mixed models. Journal of Memory and Language,59(4), 434–446.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  34. Ketrez, N. (1999). Early verbs and the acquisition of Turkish argument structure. Master’s Thesis, Boğaziçi University, İstanbul, Turkey.Google Scholar
  35. Kılıçaslan, Y. (2004). Syntax of information structure in Turkish. Linguistics,42(4), 717–765.Google Scholar
  36. Koostra, G. J., & Muysken, P. (2017). Cross-linguistic priming in bilinguals: Multidisciplinary perspectives on language processing, acquisition and change [special issue]. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition,20(2), 215–434.Google Scholar
  37. Kornfilt, J. (1997). Turkish grammar. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
  38. Kural, M. (1992). Properties of scrambling in Turkish. Manuscript. Los Angeles: UCLA.Google Scholar
  39. Kural, M. (1997). Postverbal constituents in Turkish and the linear correspondence axiom. Linguistic Inquiry,28(3), 498–519.Google Scholar
  40. Kutasi, T., Suffill, E., Gibb, C. L., Sorace, A., Pickering, M. J., & Branigan, H. P. (2018). Shared representation of passives across Scottish Gaelic and English: Evidence from structural priming. Journal of Cultural Cognitive Science.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s41809-018-0012-z.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Kutlu, A. (2015). Shared syntax in Turkish-English bilinguals. Master’s Thesis, Carleton University, Ottawa, Ontario.Google Scholar
  42. Laanemets, A. (2009). The passive voice in written and spoken Scandinavian. GAGL: Groninger Arbeiten zur germanistischen Linguistik,49, 144–166.Google Scholar
  43. Lewis, G. L. (1967). Turkish grammar. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  44. Li, P., Zhang, T., & Puls, B. (2014). Language history questionnaire (LHQ 20): A new dynamic web-based research tool. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition,17(3), 673–680.Google Scholar
  45. Lødrup, H. (2000). Exceptions to the Norwegian passive: Unaccusativity, aspect and thematic roles. Norsk Lingvistisk Tidsskrift,18(1), 37–54.Google Scholar
  46. Lødrup, H. (2013). Complex predicates in Norwegian: New evidence from passive and impersonal sentences. Paper presented in the Lexical-Functional Grammar Conference, University of Debrecen, Hungary, July 18-20, 2013.Google Scholar
  47. Loebell, H., & Bock, J. K. (2003). Structural priming across languages. Linguistics,41, 791–824.Google Scholar
  48. Mahowald, K., James, A., Futrell, R., & Gibson, E. (2016). A meta-analysis of syntactic priming in language production. Journal of Memory and Language,91, 5–27.Google Scholar
  49. McDonald, J. L., Bock, K., & Kelly, M. H. (1993). Word and world order: Semantic, phonological, and metrical determinants of serial position. Cognitive Psychology,25, 188–230.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  50. Nakipoğlu-Demiralp, M. (2001). The referential properties of the implicit arguments of impersonal passives in Turkish. Linguistik Aktuell (Linguistics today) (Vol. 44). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
  51. Özge, U., & Bozşahin, C. (2010). Intonation in the Grammar of Turkish. Lingua,120(1), 132–175.Google Scholar
  52. Özsoy, A. S. (2009). Argument structure, animacy, syntax and semantics of passivization in Turkish: A corpus-based approach. In Y. Kawaguchi, M. Minegishi, & J. Durand (Eds.), Corpus analysis and variation in linguistics (pp. 259–279). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
  53. Pickering, M. J., & Ferreira, V. S. (2008). Structural priming: A critical review. Psychological Bulletin,134(3), 427–459.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  54. Prat-Sala, M., & Branigan, H. P. (2000). Discourse constraints on syntactic processing in language production: A cross-linguistic study in English and Spanish. Journal of Memory and Language,42, 168–182.Google Scholar
  55. Sezer, E. (1979). Eylemlerin çoğul özne uyumunda anlamsal özelliklerin rolü [The role of semantic features in plural subject agreement of verbs]. Dilbilim Seçmeleri. Ankara: Ankara Üniversitesi Yayınları.Google Scholar
  56. Shin, J.-A., & Christianson, K. (2009). Processing in Korean-English bilinguals: Evidence from cross-linguistic structural priming. Cognition,112, 175–180.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  57. Sodacı, H. (2018). Role of structural priming in contact-induced change: Subject pronoun expression in NL-Turkish. Master’s Thesis, Radboud University, Nijmegen, Netherlands.Google Scholar
  58. Tarzi, M. (1983). An analysis of Turkish passive in discourse. Master’s Thesis, Boğaziçi University, İstanbul, Turkey.Google Scholar
  59. R Core Team (2018). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. https://www.R-project.org/.
  60. Vasilyeva, M., & Gámez, P. B. (2015). Exploring interactions between semantic and syntactic processes: The role of animacy in syntactic priming. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,138, 15–30.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  61. Vasilyeva, M., Waterfall, H., Gámez, P. B., Gómez, L. E., Bowers, E., & Shimpi, P. (2010). Cross-linguistic syntactic priming in bilingual children. Journal of Child Language,37, 1047–1064.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  62. Vernice, M., Pickering, M. J., & Hartsuiker, R. J. (2012). Thematic emphasis in language production. Language and Cognitive Processes,27(5), 631–664.Google Scholar
  63. Weber, K., & Indefrey, P. (2009). Syntactic priming in German-English bilinguals during sentence comprehension. Neuroimage,46(4), 1164–1172.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  64. Winter, B. (2013). Linear models and linear mixed effects models in R with linguistic applications. http://arxiv.org/pdf/1308.5499.pdf. Accessed 27 Sept 2018.
  65. Wolleb, A. (2015). Syntactic representations in the bilingual mind: the role of executive function and pragmatics in cross-language priming. PhD Thesis, UiT The Arctic University of Norway, Tromsø, NorwayGoogle Scholar
  66. Wolleb, A., Sorace, A., & Westergaard, M. (2018). Exploring the role of cognitive control in syntactic processing: Evidence from cross-language priming in bilingual children. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism,8(5), 606–636.  https://doi.org/10.1075/lab.17002.wol.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Ziegler, J., & Snedeker, J. (2018). How broad are thematic roles? Evidence from structural priming. Cognition,179, 221–240.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  68. Ziegler, J., & Snedeker, J. (2019). The use of syntax and information structure during language comprehension: Evidence from structural priming. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience,34(3), 365–384.3.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature Singapore Pte Ltd. 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Center for Multilingualism in Society Across the LifespanUniversity of OsloOsloNorway
  2. 2.Department of Foreign Language TeachingUfuk UniversityAnkaraTurkey

Personalised recommendations