Advertisement

Journal of Cultural Cognitive Science

, Volume 2, Issue 1–2, pp 85–100 | Cite as

Preliminary steps towards a cognitive theory of fiction and its effects

  • Gianluca ConsoliEmail author
Review
  • 158 Downloads

Abstract

In the last few years there has been a real explosion of studies on fiction and its effects in social, cognitive, and media psychology, in communication science, in different subdisciplines of cognitive neuroscience, in experimental aesthetics, and in the numerically aided phenomenological approach to the study of fiction. This research is at an early stage and there is general consensus that it presents conspicuous shortcomings. In this paper I expressly address one of the most relevant limitations: the lack of an interdisciplinary integration among the different trends of research. I propose a theoretical-empirical review that integrates some of the most crucial findings concerning the fictional processing and its persuasive and learning effects across disciplines. The review is presented as a network of interconnected theoretical hypotheses, based on widely shared and well-researched conceptual constructs. Each hypothesis is supported by recent relevant findings and connects different lines of research. Taken together, the hypotheses represent a preliminary step towards a cognitive theory of fiction and its effect.

Keywords

Simulation Narrative Fiction Transportation Identification Epistemic vigilance Reflection 

References

  1. Acheson, D. J., Wells, J. B., & MacDonald, M. C. (2008). New and update tests of print exposure and reading abilities in college students. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 278–289.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Altmann, U., Bohrn, I. C., Lubrich, O., Menninghaus, W., & Jacobs, A. M. (2012). Fact vs. fiction—how paratextual information shapes our reading process. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9, 22–29.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Appel, M., & Mara, M. (2013). The persuasive influence of a fictional character’s trustworthiness. Journal of Communication, 63, 912–932.Google Scholar
  4. Appel, M., & Richter, T. (2007). Persuasive effects of fictional narratives increase over time. Media Psychology, 10, 113–134.Google Scholar
  5. Bal, P. M., & Veltkamp, M. (2013). How does fiction reading influence empathy? An experimental investigation on the role of emotional transportation. PLoS ONE, 8, e55341.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bálint, K., Hakemulder, F., Kuijpers, M., Doicaru, M., & Tan, E. S. (2016). Reconceptualizing foregrounding. Identifying response strategies to deviation in absorbing narratives. Scientific Study of Literature, 6, 176–207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Bargh, J. A. (2006). What have we been priming all these years? On the development, mechanisms, and ecology of nonconscious social behavior. European Journal of Social Psychology, 36, 147–168.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Barnes, J. L. (2017). Imaginary engagement, real-world effects: Fiction, emotion, and social cognition. Review of Genera Psychology, 22, 125–134 (Advance online publication).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, Y., & Plumb, I. (2001). The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” test revised version: A study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 241–251.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. The Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 617–645.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Black, J. E., & Barnes, J. L. (2015a). The effects of reading material on social and non-social cognition. Poetics, 52, 32–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Black, J. E., & Barnes, J. L. (2015b). Fiction and social cognition: The effect of viewing award-winning television dramas on theory of mind. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 9, 423–429.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Boden, M. (2004). The creative mind: Myths and mechanism. London: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Bormann, D., & Greitemeyer, T. (2015). Immersed in virtual worlds and minds: Effects of in-game storytelling in immersion, need satisfaction, and affective theory of mind. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 6, 646–652.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Bruner, J. (1986). Actual minds, possible worlds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  16. Burke, M., Kuzmicova, A., Mangen, A., & Schilhab, T. (2016). Empathy at the confluence of neuroscience and empirical literary studies. Scientific Study of Literature, 6, 6–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Burkett, C., & Goldman, S. R. (2016). “Getting the point” of literature: Relations between processing and interpretation. Discourse Processes, 53, 457–487.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Busselle, R., & Bilandzic, H. (2008). Fictionality and perceived realism in experiencing stories: A model of narrative comprehension and engagement. Communication Theory, 18, 255–280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Busselle, R., & Bilandzic, H. (2009). Measuring narrative engagement. Media Psychology, 12, 321–347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Cohen, J. (2006). Audience identification with media characters. In J. Bryant & P. Vorderer (Eds.), Psychology of reading, Mahwah (pp. 183–198). Mahwah, N.Y: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  21. Currie, G. (1990). The nature of fiction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Currie, G. (1995). Imagination as simulation: Aesthetics meets cognitive science. In M. Davies & T. Stone (Eds.), Folk psychology: The theory of mind debate (pp. 245–268). Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
  23. Currie, G. (2001). Imagination and make-believe. In B. Gaut & D. Lopes (Eds.), The Routledge companion to aesthetics (pp. 253–263). Abingdon: Routledge.Google Scholar
  24. Currie, G. (2016). Models as fictions, fictions as models. Monist, 99, 296–310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Currie, G., & Ravenscroft, I. (2002). Recreative minds: Imagination in philosophy and psychology. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Dal Cin, S., Stoolmiller, M., & Sargent, J. D. (2012). When movies matter: exposure to smoking in movies and changes in smoking behavior. Journal Health Communication, 17, 76–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Dal Cin, S., Zanna, M. P., & Fong, G. T. (2004). Narrative persuasion and overcoming resistance. In E. S. Knowles & J. A. Linn (Eds.), Resistance and persuasion (pp. 175–191). Mahwah, NJ, US: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.Google Scholar
  28. De Vignemont, F. (2009). Drawing the boundary between low-level and high-level mindreading. Philosophical Studies, 144, 457–466.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. De Vignemont, F., & Singer, T. (2006). The empathic brain: How, when and why”. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 435–441.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Djikic, M., Oatley, K., & Carland, M. (2012). Genre or artistic merit: The effect of literature on personality. Scientific Study of Literature, 2, 25–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Djikic, M., Oatley, K., Zoeterman, S., & Peterson, J. B. (2009). On being moved by art: How reading fiction transforms the self. Creativity Research Journal, 21, 24–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Fayn, K., MacCann, C., Tiliopoulos, N., & Silvia, P. J. (2015). Aesthetic emotions and aesthetic people: Openness predicts sensitivity to novelty in the experiences of interest and pleasure. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1877.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Fazio, R., & Towles-Schwen, T. (1999). The mode model of attitude-behavior processes. In S. Chaiken & Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual-process theories in social psychology (pp. 97–116). New York: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  34. Fludernick, M. (2006). An introduction to narratology. New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
  35. Fong, K., Mullin, J. B., & Mar, R. A. (2013). What you read matters: The role of fiction genre in predictiong interpersonal sensitivity. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 7, 370–376.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Galgut, E. (2014). Harnessing the imagination: The asymmetry of belief and make-believe. Contemporary Aesthetics, 12, 21.Google Scholar
  37. Gendler, T. (2011). Imagination, The Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy.Google Scholar
  38. Gernot, G., Pelowski, M., & Leder, H. (2017). Empathy, Einfühlung, and esthetic experience: The effect of emotion contagion on appreciation of representational and abstract art using fEMG and SCR. Cognitive Processing, 19, 147–165.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-017-0800-2. (Epub ahead of print).CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Gerrig, R. J. (1993). Experiencing narrative worlds: On the psychological activities of reading. New York: Haven UP.Google Scholar
  40. Gerrig, R. J., & Wenzel, W. G. (2015). The role of inferences in narrative experiences. In E. J. O’Brien, A. E. Cook, & R. F. Lorch (Eds.), Inferences during reading (pp. 362–385). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Giles, D. (2002). Parasocial interaction: A review of the literature and a model for future research. Media Psychology, 4, 279–305.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Green, M. C. (2004). Transportation into narrative worlds: The role of prior knowledge and perceived realism. Discourse Processes, 38, 247–266.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Green, M. C., & Brock, T. C. (2000). The role of transportation in the persuasiveness of public narratives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 701–721.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Green, M. C., & Brock, T. C. (2002). In the mind’s eye: Transportation-imagery model of narrative persuasion. In M. C. Green, J. J. Strange, & T. C. Brock (Eds.), Narrative impact: Social and cognitive foundations (pp. 315–341). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  45. Green, M., & Clark, J. (2012). Transportation into narrative worlds: Implications for entertainment media influences on tobacco use. Addiction Review, 108, 477–484.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Hakemulder, F. (2007). Tracing foregrounding in responses to film. Language and Literature, 16, 125–139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Hakemulder, F., & van Peer, W. (2015). Empirical stylistics. In V. Sotirova (Ed.), A companion to stylistics (pp. 251–274). London: Continuum.Google Scholar
  48. Hall, M. P., O’Hare, A., Santavicca, N., & Jones, L. F. (2015). The power of deep reading and mindful literacy; An innovative approach in contemporary education. Innovación Educativa, 15, 49–60.Google Scholar
  49. Herman, D. (2002). Problems and possibilities of narrative. Nebraska: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
  50. Hynyard, C., & Kreuter, M. (2007). Using narrative communication as a tool for health behavior change: A conceptual, theoretical, and empirical overview. Health Education Behavior, 34, 777–792.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Iguarta, J. J. (2010). Identification with characters and narrative persuasion through fictional feature films. The European Journal of Communication Research, 35, 347–373.Google Scholar
  52. Johnson, D. R. (2012). Transportation into a story increases empathy, prosocial behavior, and perceptual bias toward fearful expressions. Personality and Individual Differences, 53, 150–155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Kidd, D. C., & Castano, E. (2013). Reading literary fiction improves theory of mind. Science, 342, 377–380.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Kidd, D. C., & Castano, E. (2016). Different stories: How levels of familiarity with literary and genre fiction relate to mentalizing. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts., 11, 1–13.Google Scholar
  55. Kidd, D. C., & Castano, E. (2018). Reading literary fiction and theory of mind: Three preregistered replications and extension of Kidd and Castano (2013). Social Psychological and Personality Science, 20, 1–10.Google Scholar
  56. Koopman, E. M. E. (2015a). Empathic reactions after reading. The role of genre, personal factors and affective responses. Poetics, 50, 62–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Koopman, E. M. E. (2015b). How texts about suffering trigger reflection: Genre, personal factors, and affective responses. Journal of Aesthetics, Creativity and the Arts, 9, 430–441.Google Scholar
  58. Koopman, E. M. E. (2016). Effects of ‘literariness’ on emotions and on empathy and reflection after reading. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 9, 430–441.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Koopman, E. M. E., & Hakemulder, F. (2015). Effects of literature on empathy and self-reflection: A theoretical-empirical framework. Journal of Literary Theory, 9, 79–111-.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Kotovych, M., Dixon, P., Bortolussi, M., & Holden, M. (2011). Textual determinants of a component of literary identification. Scientific Study of Literature, 1, 260–291.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Kreuter, M., Green, M., Cappella, J., Slater, M., Wise, M., Storey, D., et al. (2007). Narrative communication in cancer prevention and control: A framework to guide research and application. Annual Behavioral Medicine, 33, 221–235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Kuiken, D., Miall, D., & Sikora, S. (2004). Forms of self-implication in literary reading. Poetics Today, 25, 171–203.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Leder, H., Gerger, G., Brieber, D., & Schwarz, N. (2014). What makes an art expert? Emotions and evaluation in art appreciation. Emotion and Cognition, 28, 1–11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Locher, P., Overbeeke, K., & Wensveen, S. (2010). Aesthetic interaction: A framework. Des Issues, 26, 70–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Mar, R. A. (2011). The neural basis of social cognition and story comprehension. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 103–134.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Mar, R. A. (2018). Stories and the promotion of social cognition. Current Direction in Psychological Science, 27, 257–262.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Mar, R. A., & Oatley, K. (2008). The function of fiction is the abstraction and simulation of social experience. Perspective on Psychological Science, 3, 173–192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Mar, R. A., Oatley, K., Hirsh, J., dela Paz, J., & Peterson, J. B. (2006). Bookworms versus nerds: Exposure to fiction versus non-fiction, divergent associations, with social ability, and the simulation of fictional social worlds. Journal of Research in Personality, 40, 649–712.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Mar, R. A., Oatley, K., & Person, J. B. (2009). Exploring the link between reading fiction and empathy: Ruling out individual differences and examining outcomes. Communications: The European Journal of Communication Research, 34, 407–428.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Marsh, E. J., Meade, M. L., & Roediger, H. L. (2003). Learning facts from fiction. Journal of Memory and Language, 49, 519–536.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Menninghaus, W., Wagner, V., Hanich, J., Wassiliwizky, E., Jacobsen, T., & Koelsch, S. (2017). The distancing-embracing model of the enjoyment of negative emotions in art reception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 40, E347.  https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X17000309.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  72. Miall, D., & Kuiken, D. (1994). Foregrounding, defamiliarization, and affect: Response to literary stories. Poetics, 22, 389–407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Miall, D., & Kuiken, D. (1999). What is literariness? Three components of literary reading. Discourse Processes, 28, 121–128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Miall, D., & Kuiken, D. (2002). A feeling for fiction: Becoming what we behold. Poetics, 30, 221–241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Morgenstern, M., Poelen, E. A., Scholte, R., Karlsdottir, S., Johnsson, S. H., Mathis, E., et al. (2011). Smoking in movies and adolescent smoking: Cross-cultural study in six European countries. Thorax, 66, 875–883.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Moyer-Gusé, E. (2008). Toward a theory of entertainment persuasion: Explaining the persuasive effects of entertainment-education messages. Communication Theory, 18, 407–425.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Mukařovský, J. (1976). On poetic language. Lisse: De Ridder Press.Google Scholar
  78. Mumper, M. L., & Gerrig, R. J. (2017). Liesure reading and social cognition. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 11, 109–120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Nichols, S. (2006). Just the imagination: Why imagining doesn’t behave like believing. Mind and Language, 21, 459–474.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  80. Oatley, K. (2016). Fiction: Simulation of social worlds. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20, 618–628.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  81. Oatley, K., & Djikic, M. (2017). The creativity of literary writing. In J. Kaufman, J. Baer, & V. Glaveanu (Eds.), Cambridge handbook of creativity across domains. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  82. Oatley, K., Dunbar, R., & Budelmann, F. (2018). Imagining possible worlds. Review of General Psychology, 22, 121–124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  83. Origgi, G. (2013). Epistemic injustice and epistemic trust. Social Epistemology: A Journal of Knowledge, Culture and Policy, 26, 221–235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  84. Panero, M. E., Weisberg, D. S., Black, J., Goldstein, T. R., Barnes, J. L., Brownell, H., et al. (2016). Does reading a single passage of literary fiction really improve theory of mind? An attempt at replication. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 111, 46–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  85. Pelowski, M. (2015). Tears and transformation: Feeling like crying as an indicator of insightful or ‘aesthetic’ experience in empirical study of art. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  86. Pelowski, M., Markey, P., Forster, M., Gernot, G., & Helmut, L. (2017). Move me, astonish me…delight my eyes and brain: The Vienna integrated model of top-down and bottom up processes in art perception and corresponding affective, evaluative, and neurophysiological correlates. Physics of Life Reviews, 21, 80–125.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  87. Peretti, S. et al. (2018). The role of sleep in aesthetic perception and empathy: A mediation analysis. Journal of Sleep Research.  https://doi.org/10.1111/jsr.12664 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  88. Petty, R. E., & Wegener, D. T. (1999). The elaboration likelihood model: Current status and controversies. In S. Chaiken & Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual-process theories in social psychology (pp. 41–72). New York: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  89. Pino, M. C., & Mazza, M. (2016). The use of “literary fiction” to promote mentalizing ability. PLoS One, 11(8), e0160254.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0160254 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  90. Richter, T., Appel, M., & Calio, F. (2014). Stories can influence the self-concept. Social Influence, 9, 172–188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  91. Samur, D., Tops, M., & Koole, S. L. (2017). Does a single session of reading literary fiction prime enhanced mentalizing performance? Four replication experiments of Kidd and Castano (2013). Cognition and Emotion, 32, 130–144.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  92. Schaeffer, J. M. (1999). Pourquoi la fiction?. Paris: Seul.Google Scholar
  93. Shklovsky, V. (1965). Art as technique. In L. T. Lemon & M. J. Reis (Eds.), Russian formalist criticism: Four essays (pp. 3–24). University of Nebraska Press: Lincoln.Google Scholar
  94. Sikora, S., Kuiken, D., & Miall, D. (2010). An uncommon resonance: The influence of loss on expressive reading. Empirical Studies of the Arts, 28, 135–153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  95. Silvia, J. P. (2013). Interested experts, confused novices: Art expertise and the knowledge emotions. Empirical Studies of the Arts, 31, 107–115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  96. Slater, M. D., & Rouner, D. (2002). Entertaintment-education and elaboration likelihood: Understanding the processing of narrative persuasion. Communication Theory, 12, 173–191.Google Scholar
  97. Sperber, D., Clément, F., Heintz, C., Mascaro, O., Mercier, H., Origgi, G., et al. (2010). Epistemic vigilance. Mind and Language, 25, 359–393.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  98. Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (2000). Individual differences in reasoning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 2000(23), 701–726.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  99. Tamir, D. I., Bricker, A. B., Dodell-Feder, D., & Mitchell, J. P. (2016). Reading fiction and reading minds: the role of simulation in the default network. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11, 215–224.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  100. Tomasello, M. (2015). The ultra-social animal. European Journal of Social Psychology, 44, 187–194.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  101. Van Peer, W., Hakemulder, J., & Zyngier, S. (2007). Lines on feeling: Foregrounding, aesthetics, and meaning. Language and Literature, 16, 197–213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  102. Vezzali, L., Stathi, S., Giovannini, D., Capozza, D., & Trifiletti, E. (2015). The greatest magic of Harry Potter: Reducing prejudice. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 45, 105–121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  103. Walton, K. (1990). Mimesis as make-believe. Harvard: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  104. Zwan, R. A. (2004). The immersed experience. Toward an embodied theory of language comprehension. In B. H. Ross (Ed.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation (pp. 35–62). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature Singapore Pte Ltd. 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Social and Developmental PsychologyLa Sapienza, University of RomeRomeItaly

Personalised recommendations